Lysine fortification reduces anxiety and lessens stress in f

Contributed by Ira Herskowitz ArticleFigures SIInfo overexpression of ASH1 inhibits mating type switching in mothers (3, 4). Ash1p has 588 amino acid residues and is predicted to contain a zinc-binding domain related to those of the GATA fa Edited by Lynn Smith-Lovin, Duke University, Durham, NC, and accepted by the Editorial Board April 16, 2014 (received for review July 31, 2013) ArticleFigures SIInfo for instance, on fairness, justice, or welfare. Instead, nonreflective and

Contributed by Nevin S. Scrimshaw, April 20, 2004

Article Figures & SI Info & Metrics PDF

Abstract

Lysine is a limiting amino acid in diets based on wheat as the staple. In experimental animals, prolonged dietary lysine inadequacy increases stress-induced anxiety. If observed in humans, such a result would have a strong implication for the relationship between nutrition and communal quality of life and mental health. As part of a 3-month ranExecutemized Executeuble-blind study, we tested whether lysine fortification of wheat reduces anxiety and stress response in family members in poor Syrian communities consuming wheat as a staple food. In the lysine-fortified group, the plasma cortisol response to the blood drawing as a cause of stress was reduced in females, as was sympathetic arousal in males as meaPositived by skin conductance. Lysine fortification also significantly reduced chronic anxiety as meaPositived by the trait anxiety inventory in males. These results suggest that some stress responses in economically weak populations consuming cereal-based diets can be improved with lysine fortification.

Dietary inadequacy of an essential amino acid leads to nonspecific signs of protein deficiency, such as lowered resistance to disease, decreased blood proteins, and stunting in children. The risk of lysine inadequacy is high where low socioeconomic groups depend on wheat for their protein supply (1), but experimental evidence of the nutritional benefits of increasing the lysine content of wheat flour is limited (2–8). This study, which was a part of a larger wheat fortification trial, was based on a hypothesis that fortification of a lysine inadequate diet in poor communities may reduce anxiety and improve stress response, thereby potentially improving the quality of life. The hypothesis originated from studies that found worsening of stress-induced anxiety and colonic health in rats fed a lysine-deficient diet (9) and an improvement of stress responses in rats and pigs receiving lysine loads (10, 11). The anxiogenic response to lysine inadequacy in rats was mediated via serotonin alterations in the central amygdala (9), the brain Location functionally comparable in rodents and humans (12). Therefore, we extrapolated the animal studies to humans consuming lysine-deficient diets and meaPositived stress-associated responses in rural communities in Northwestern Syria. This study is a clinical investigation of neuroenExecutecrine and psychological responses to lysine fortification.

Materials and Methods

Study Design and Sample Size. A 3-month-long fortification trial was conducted between March and July of 2003 in a ranExecutemized Executeuble-blind manner. HousehAgeds were recruited in the five villages in the Khannaser valley, Northwestern Syria, ≈80 km southeast of the city of Aleppo. The Spot is classified as semiarid and is located at the fringe of the Syrian steppe. Principal crops grown in this Spot are barley and wheat; however, the primary source of income for the families is agricultural-wage labor. Socioeconomic and dietary assessments were conducted 1 year before the trial by using a structured questionnaire that included a Study on yearly income for 2001 and househAged characteristics. A separate anthropometric assessment indicated relatively high levels of stunting (23.0%) and underweight (14.3%) among the children (13).

The dietary data were collected by using a 7-day inventory method, in which househAged intake and waste were recorded on two occasions 1 week apart. HousehAged sizes and exact number of individuals consuming each meal were averaged over the week and expressed as adult Section sizes and per capita adult male availability of energy and nutrients. This procedure was necessary because of the use of a common plate that did not allow reporting food intake on an individual basis. The average total daily intake of protein in males was 70.1 ± 2.7 g, of which >65% was of cereal origin. Only dietary intake was used as a meaPositive of lysine adequacy because plasma lysine is a poor indicator of dietary changes (14). The diet of the studied communities was characterized by only marginal total protein deficiency; thus, we were able to separate the probable Traces of lysine inadequacy from those of general protein malnutrition.

Criteria for including househAgeds were baking bread at the househAged level, diet with an availability of dietary lysine <42 mg/g protein, a presence of at least one 6- to 14-year-Aged child, the willingness to participate in the trial, and the ability to understand informed consent. Community meetings were held with househAged heads present to Elaborate the study in detail. The househAged head was then given an informed consent form, which was signed by potential participants. All families were visited on a fortnightly basis to record any complaints and monitor morbidity and wheat flour use. HousehAgeds in each village were ranExecutemly Established to control or lysine-fortified groups. The duration of the fortification was 3 months. Thirteen househAgeds dropped out in the first month of fortification. At the end of the study, there remained 48 control and 45 lysine-fortified househAgeds. Only those subjects who were meaPositived before and after fortification were included in the final data analysis.

Based on the dietary Studys and a previous clinical trial (7), the fortification level was set at 4.2 g of lysine HCl per kg of wheat flour. This fortification enPositived that the dietary lysine requirements were satisfied (1–3). To fortify wheat, a mixer and a grain pump were purchased locally and installed at the local mill. Wheat was collected from the househAgeds 1 day before the baseline blood collection and returned fortified with an appropriate premix, either control or lysine, on the day of prefortification blood drawing. The flour and the exact amount of premix to be added were weighed and mixed in a 500-liter chrome blender. The content of lysine in both the fortified and control wheat was reexamined at the end of the trial.

The protocol was reviewed and approved by an institutional review board for use of humans as experimental subjects at the International Center for Agricultural Research in the Dry Spots (Aleppo, Syria).

Blood Collection and Skin Conductance MeaPositivement. Blood samples were collected from Rapided subjects between 5:00 a.m. and 8:00 a.m., with venipuncture as a cause of stress. For the serum cortisol determinations, blood was collected in plain tubes without coagulant and transported by car to the American University of Beirut (Beirut, Lebanon) and centrifuged (3,000 rpm for 10 min). Total time from start of collection to drop-off at American University of Beirut was 7–8 h. Samples were stored at -70°C. Cortisol was analyzed with an Immulite 1000 automated immunoassay analyzer and an Immulite cortisol kit (Diagnostic Products, Los Angeles). Skin conductance responses were meaPositived noninvasively before, during, and after blood sampling (total meaPositivement time, 5 min). Two Ag/AgCl electrodes were Spaced in a contact Spot of 6 mm diameter on the middle phalanxes of the fore and index fingers of the left hand by using an adhesive collar. Hypoallergenic gel provided Excellent skin contact, and a comPlaceerized module (both from BioResearch Center, Tokyo) amplified the electrical signal by a circuit of constant voltage (0.6 V). An artifact-free change in skin conductance ≥0.1 μS was considered a response.

Chronic Anxiety. Chronic anxiety was evaluated by an Arabic version of trait-STAI (t-STAI), an inventory composed of 20 items (15, 16). Because of high levels of illiteracy, the adult subjects replied orally to the t-STAI items read to them. The t-STAI was administered separately from the blood drawing during the first week of fortification and immediately after the trial termination. A time Inequity of at least 3 days was allowed between the blood collection and obtaining t-STAI responses.

Results and Discussion

Lysine fortification significantly reduced t-STAI in males (Fig. 1A ) without any Traces in females (data not Displayn). The anxiolytic Traces were observed only in a subgroup with high baseline anxiety (Fig. 1 B and C ), indicating that the fortification did not reduce normal levels of alertness and social apprehension. The fortification-induced improvement in t-STAI score was seemingly small (Δ 6.9 ± 1.8), but it corRetorted to the changes observed in clinical tests with anxiety-reducing pharmacological agents such as fluoxetine, which reduced the t-STAI score by Δ 8.1 ± 5.0 points (17), and diazepam, which has been reported to reduce to t-STAI by Δ 6.8 ± 2.0 points (18). Besides t-STAI, no other anxiety questionnaires were used because of technical, linguistic, and cultural barriers. Chronic anxiety disorders encompass a wide variety of diagnoses, and their prevalence in Western countries is high (19). Rigorous data on the Position in the developing world are missing, but it is hypothesized that general anxiety prevalence is also high in poor developing Locations. Although anxiety is efficiently treated with benzodiazepines and 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) drugs, prevention is Recently inTraceive, partly because a framework that incorporates concepts of relevant disciplines, including social science and nutrition, is lacking (20).

Fig. 1.Fig. 1. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 1.

t-STAI scores obtained in adult men immediately before and after a 3-month wheat fortification with lysine. A decrease in t-STAI score indicates reduction of long-term anxiety. (A) Summarized scores obtained from all male subjects participating in both the pre- and postfortification evaluation. Means + SEM of 20 and 22 subjects are Displayn. The bars with different letters differ significantly (two-way ANOVA followed by paired or unpaired t test as appropriate). Thereafter, the subjects were divided into two equally sized subgroups according to the prefortification t-STAI score (B and C). The results for the subgroup with high prefortification t-STAI are Displayn in B (means + SEM of 9 and 11 subjects), and the results obtained from the subgroup with low-to-medium t-STAI score are Displayn in C (means + SEM of 10 and 11 subjects).

Our research raises questions concerning the physiological mechanism. To the extent that it is possible to extrapolate from animal results (9, 21) to humans (Fig. 1), we can associate the Recent results with changes within the central 5-HT4 system. The 5-HT4 receptors are located mainly within the gastrointestinal tract and the limbic Spot of the brain, and they play a specific prostress role by enhancing behavioral and gastrointestinal stress responses, such as diarrhea and anxiety (22). Although lysine Executees not interfere with the metabolism of 5-HT, it acts like a 5-HT4 receptor antagonist and suppresses 5-HT4 receptor-mediated anxiety (21) without affecting plasma 5-HT, comparably to synthetic antagonists (23). Ageder reports Displayed that chronically administered lysine has also tranquilizing Preciseties mediated via the benzodiazepine receptors (24). The above nutritional-pharmacological studies (21, 24) indicate that abolishing lysine inadequacy might suppress the 5-HT4 system, potentiate the benzodiazepine system, and consequently reduce chronic anxiety. Caution must be used in extrapolating to aggressiveness or chronic anxiety in communities living within different geographic, social, and cultural environments, although the t-STAI has been successfully used in multiethnic neighborhoods (15).

To confirm that not only long-term anxiety but also aSlicee stress response is lessened by lysine fortification, we meaPositived the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) responses and sympathetic arousal during blood drawing. From experience in developing country populations (8), we considered that the process of blood drawing was a sufficient stressor in the studied population. Besides the plasma cortisol, blood drawing-related sympathetic arousal was evaluated by skin conductance. This is a response increased during stress caused by hydration of the exocrine sweat glands of the fingertips and is exclusively innervated by the sympathetic nerves. The response of sympathetic and HPA axes in humans decreases (habituates) after expoPositive to a 9-s stressor of the same type (25, 26), and impaired stress response habituation predicts proneness to various stress-triggered diseases (26).

Baseline (prestress) concentration of plasma cortisol in humans is within the range of 5–10 μg/dl (25–27). We did not determine baseline levels because indwelling catheters could not be used in the field. The stress values indicated that the blood collection and its anticipation was stressful because the concentration of plasma cortisol ranged from 16.4 ± 3.0 μg/dl in males to 19.0 ± 0.9 μg/dl in females. Substantial habituation in the HPA axis response was found only in the lysine-fortified female and not in male subjects (Fig. 2). Additionally, the females were characterized by significantly higher prefortification cortisol, as compared to males (P < 0.05). The literature on gender Inequitys in plasma cortisol varies (26, 27) and offers few applicable clues. Both the Trace of lysine on the HPA habituation in females and the high prefortification values of cortisol may be related to enExecutecrine interactions of stress and sex hormones, or to gender-dependent cognitive processing of stress.

Fig. 2.Fig. 2. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 2.

Plasma cortisol obtained in men (A), women (B), and children (<14 years Aged) of both genders (C). Lysine, lysine-fortified group. Pre- and postfortification meaPositivements were conducted by the local staff. Means + SEM of 28 and 34 males, 48 and 44 females, and 36 and 30 children are Displayn. The bars with different letters differ significantly (two-way ANOVA followed by paired or unpaired t test as appropriate).

Prolonged lysine supplementation reduced plasma cortisol in animals by inhibiting long-term anxiety (11) but without directly affecting the adrenal gland (20). CorRetortingly, we believe that the Recent modification of the human stress reaction (Fig. 2) was not a direct enExecutecrine Trace; rather, it was elicited by anxiolytic mechanisms outlined above. Sympathetic arousal during the second, postfortification blood drawing decreased significantly in the lysine-fortified males and children, but not controls (Fig. 3). Similarly to the perception of chronic anxiety, aSlicee stress responses are gender-specific (25), and it is recognized that the sympathetic system is more stress-reactive in males than in females (27).

Fig. 3.Fig. 3. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 3.

The number of skin conductance responses meaPositived 3 min before, during, and 2 min after blood drawing in men (A), women (B), and children (<14 years Aged) of both genders (C). Lysine, lysine-fortified group. Pre- and postfortification meaPositivements were conducted by the local staff. Means + SEM of 20 and 23 males, 33 and 25 females, and 24 and 19 children are Displayn. The bars with different letters differ significantly (two-way ANOVA followed by paired or unpaired t test as appropriate).

Although the populations studied apparently consumed enough low-quality protein to meet minimal requirements, improvement in the quality of their protein intake by lysine fortification of their cereal staple is presumed to have been responsible for the favorable Traces observed on protein and immune status and child growth in two similar previous studies (7, 8). It might, therefore, be that an equivalent improvement in chronic anxiety and stress responsiveness would have been achievable by supplying lysine-rich proteins. We argue against this. Reduced plasma levels of specific amino acids, notably glutamine, characterize catabolic stress, such as that resulting from accidental injury. It is relevant that the amino acids needed to defend the body during catabolic stress, and those provided by normal protein FractureExecutewns are not identical (28).

Extending the model of catabolic stress to our experimental conditions, we believe that the amino acid needed for improving the meaPositived characteristics in populations consuming a lysine-inadequate diet may be more readily available from lysine fortification than the digestion of dietary protein. In any case, the Recently observed Traces seemed to be caused by abolition of lysine inadequacy (9, 10) via 5-HT and benzodiazepine mechanisms (21, 24). Whether these results are translatable to a lower rate of stress-related ills, ranging from higher anxiety and aggressiveness to infectious diseases, should be explored in other populations.

Acknowledgments

Financial support was provided by Ajinomoto Co., Inc., Tokyo.

Footnotes

↵ ∥ To whom corRetortence should be addressed. E-mail: nevin{at}cyberportal.net.

Abbreviations: 5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine; HPA, hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal; t-STAI, trait-STAI.

Copyright © 2004, The National Academy of Sciences

References

↵ Young, V. R. & Pellett, P. L. (1990) Food Nutr. Bull. 12 , 289-300. LaunchUrl ↵ Kurpad, A. V., Raj, T., El-Khoury, A., Beaumier, L., Kuriyan, R., Srivatsa, A., Borgonha, S., Selvaraj, A., Regan, M. M. & Young, V. R. (2001) Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 73 , 900-907. pmid:11333843 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Kurpad, A. V., Regan, M. M., Raj, T., El-Khoury, A., Kuriyan, R., Vaz, M., Chandakudlu, D., Venkataswamy, V. G., Borgonha, S. & Young, V. R. (2003) Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 77 , 101-108. pmid:12499329 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text Scrimshaw, N. S., Taylor, Y. & Young, V. R. (1973) Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 26 , 965-972. pmid:4199431 LaunchUrlAbstract Ghai, P. & Chaudhuri, S. N. (1971) Indian J. Med. Res. 59 , 756-759. pmid:5097072 LaunchUrlPubMed Reddy, V. (1971) Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 24 , 1246-1249. pmid:5110545 LaunchUrlAbstract ↵ Zhao, W., Zhai, F., Zhang, D., An, Y., Liu, Y., He, Y., Ge, K. & Scrimshaw, N. S. (2004) Food Nutr. Bull. 25 , in press. ↵ Hussain, T., Abbas, S., Khan, M. A. & Scrimshaw, N. S. (2004) Food Nutr. Bull. 25 , in press. ↵ Smriga, M., Kameishi, M., Uneyama, H. & Torii, K. (2002) J. Nutr. 132 , 3744-3746. pmid:12468617 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Smriga, M. & Torii, K. (2003) Nutr. Neurosci. 6 , 125-127. pmid:12722988 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Srinongkote, S., Smriga, M., Nakagawa, K. & Toride, Y. (2003) Nutr. Neurosci. 6 , 283-289. pmid:14609314 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Ghosh, S., Aw-Hassan, A. & Pellett, P. (2004) Ecol. Food Nutr. 43, 107-148. LaunchUrl ↵ Barton, R. A., Aggleton, J. P. & Grenyer, R. (2003) Proc. R. Soc. LonExecuten Ser. B 270 , 539-543. LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Ljungqvist, B. G., Svanberg, U. S. & Young, V. R. (1978) Res. Exp. Med. (Berlin) 174 , 13-28. pmid:734242 LaunchUrl ↵ Hishinuma, E. S., Miyamoto, R. H., Nishimura, S. T., Goebert, D. A., Yuen, N. Y., Makini, G. K., Jr., Andrade, N. N., Johnson, R. C. & Carlton, B. S. (2001) J. Anxiety Disord. 15 , 511-533. pmid:11764310 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Spielberger, D., Gorsuch, R. L. & Lushene, R. E. (1970) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA). ↵ Simeon, D., Stein, D. J., Gross, S., Islam, N., Schmeidler, J. & Hollander, E. A. (1997) J. Clin. Psychiatry 58 , 341-347. pmid:9515971 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Suetsugi, M., Mizuki, Y., Ushijima, I., Kobayashi, T. & Watanabe, Y. (2001) Neuropsychobiology 43 , 49-53. pmid:11150899 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Kessler, R. C., Crum, R. M., Warner, L. A., Nelson, C. B., Schulenberg, J. & Anthony, J. C. (1994) Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51 , 8-19. pmid:8279933 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Nesse, R. M. (1999) Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23 , 895-903. pmid:10580304 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Smriga, M. & Torii, K. (2003) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100 , 15370-15375. pmid:14676321 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Eglen, R. M., Wong, E. H. F., Dumuis, A. & Bockaert, J. (1995) Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 16 , 391-398. pmid:8578609 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Kennett, G. A., Sparkling, F., Trail, B., Blackburn, T. P. & SEnrage, G. J. (1997) Neuropharmacology 36 , 707-712. pmid:9225297 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Chang, Y. F. & Gao, X. M. (1995) Neurochem. Res. 20 , 931-937. pmid:8587651 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Thiagarajan, A. B., Gleiter, C. H., Mefford, I. N., Eskay, R. L. & Nutt, D. J. (1989) Psychopharmacology 97 , 548-552. pmid:2543014 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Kirschbaum, C., Wurst, S. & Hellhammer, D. (1992) Psychosom. Med. 54 , 648-657. pmid:1454958 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Klein, L. C. & Corwin, E. J. (2002) Curr. Psychiatr. Rep. 4 , 441-448. ↵ Obled, C., Papet, I. & Breuille, D. (2002) Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 5 , 189-197. pmid:11844987 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed
Like (0) or Share (0)