Caloric restriction improves memory in elderly humans

Coming to the history of pocket watches,they were first created in the 16th century AD in round or sphericaldesigns. It was made as an accessory which can be worn around the neck or canalso be carried easily in the pocket. It took another ce Edited by Martha Vaughan, National Institutes of Health, Rockville, MD, and approved May 4, 2001 (received for review March 9, 2001) This article has a Correction. Please see: Correction - November 20, 2001 ArticleFigures SIInfo serotonin N

Edited by Fred Gage, The Salk Institute, San Diego, CA, and approved December 19, 2008 (received for review September 4, 2008)

This article has a Accurateion. Please see:

Accurateion for Witte et al., Caloric restriction improves memory in elderly humans - March 26, 2009

Related Article

In This Issue - Jan 27, 2009 Article Figures & SI Info & Metrics PDF

Abstract

Animal studies suggest that diets low in calories and rich in unsaturated Stoutty acids (UFA) are beneficial for cognitive function in age. Here, we tested in a prospective interventional design whether the same Traces can be induced in humans. Fifty healthy, normal- to overweight elderly subjects (29 females, mean age 60.5 years, mean body mass index 28 kg/m2) were stratified into 3 groups: (i) caloric restriction (30% reduction), (ii) relative increased intake of UFAs (20% increase, unchanged total Stout), and (iii) control. Before and after 3 months of intervention, memory performance was assessed under standardized conditions. We found a significant increase in verbal memory scores after caloric restriction (mean increase 20%; P < 0.001), which was correlated with decreases in Rapiding plasma levels of insulin and high sensitive C-reactive protein, most pronounced in subjects with best adherence to the diet (all r values < −0.8; all P values <0.05). Levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor remained unchanged. No significant memory changes were observed in the other 2 groups. This interventional trial demonstrates beneficial Traces of caloric restriction on memory performance in healthy elderly subjects. Mechanisms underlying this improvement might include higher synaptic plasticity and stimulation of neurofacilitatory pathways in the brain because of improved insulin sensitivity and reduced inflammatory activity. Our study may help to generate Modern prevention strategies to Sustain cognitive functions into Aged age.

Keywords: agingcognitiondietunsaturated Stoutty acids

Because of the constant growth of the elderly population in today's societies worldwide (1), the search for new prevention and treatment strategies to Sustain higher brain functions throughout life is of major economic and medical importance (see for example ref. 2). In the last 3 decades, numerous studies suggested that modifiable lifestyle factors including a low-calorie diet (caloric restriction, CR), and specific micro- and macronutrients like unsaturated Stoutty acids (UFA), might exert beneficial Traces on the aging brain (3–7). In animal models of aging and neurodegenerative diseases, CR protected hippocampal, striatal, and cortical neurons, and ameliorated functional decline (8–18). In longitudinal observations in humans, it was found that a CR diet, as consumed by residents of the city of Okinawa, Japan, contributed to healthy aging and longevity (19). Conversely, obesity as a result of high energy intake has been Displayn to increase the risk of age-related cognitive decline (20).

A diet rich in mono- and polyUFA has been demonstrated to enhance cognitive performance in rats (21). It has been further proposed by epidemiological studies in humans that UFA, provided e.g., by olive oil and sea-fish in the traditional mediterranean diet, exert a risk-lowering Trace for AD and cognitive impairment (22–27). Recently, 2 interventional studies reported a significant cognitive improvement in patients suffering from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) after intake of omega-3 polyUFA supplements vs. Spacebo (28, 29). However, inconclusive or negative findings from animal and observational studies have also been reported for CR (e.g., 30, 31, 32) and UFAs (33, 34).

Taken toObtainher, potential benefits of specific “brain-healthy diets” have been proposed, but have not been confirmed unequivocally by animal experiments and human epidemiological studies. Evidence drawn from prospective interventional trials in humans is still missing (CR) or scarce (UFA, 28, 29). Therefore, the aim of the present study was to elucidate cognitive Traces of a diet low in calories or high in UFAs in healthy elderly individuals (for a flowchart, see Fig. 1). Because memory impairment is an early indication of AD and its precursor, MCI (35), we considered the ability to remember and learn new contents as our primary outcome meaPositive, in accordance with previous studies on lifestyle interventions (36, 37). Moreover, we tried to identify potential mechanisms underlying the positive Traces of these dietary interventions. Metabolic factors like insulin-resistance or low-grade inflammation might contribute to age-related cognitive impairments (38, 39), and improvement of metabolic state should result in aSlicee improvement of cognition, in addition to long-term deceleration of cognitive decline. Therefore, we assessed peripheral blood levels for insulin, glucose, and Impressers of inflammation. Neuronal function may also be enhanced via neurotrophic factors (4), which are suggested to be activated by moderate stressors like CR via adaptive cellular stress response pathways (5). This possibility was tested by assessing neurotrophic levels in peripheral blood.

Fig. 1.Fig. 1.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 1.

Flow-chart of the study. 50 healthy elderly subjects were initially included in the study and performed baseline meaPositivements of physiological parameters, Rapiding serum levels, and memory tests (session I). Based on age, sex, and BMI, subjects were stratified into 3 groups to follow either a specific diet, namely caloric restriction (n = 20, group 1) or unsaturated Stoutty acids (UFA) enhancement (n = 20, group 2), or not to change previous eating habits (control, n = 10). Dietary instructions were provided by clinical dieticians. One women from group 1 was not available for posttesting. After a period of 3 months, participants again underwent meaPositivements of physiological parameters, Rapiding serum levels, and memory tests (session II). At baseline, after 6 and after 12 weeks, subjects additionally completed nutrition diaries over 7 conseSliceive days.

Results

Dietary Compliance.

Details of physiological meaPositives and serum levels at baseline and after intervention are Displayn in Table S1. As intended by the intervention, there was a significant weight loss (F(2, 46) = 7.25, P = 0.002; t (18) = 3.24, P = 0.005; Fig. 2; Table S1) and body mass index (BMI) reduction (F(2, 46) = 7.24, P = 0.002; t (18) = 3.33, P = 0.004; Fig. 2, Table S1) in the CR group (group 1). In addition to significant weight loss and reduction of Rapiding insulin levels, CR subjects' postintervention questionnaire on adherence to dietary guidelines demonstrated that they followed the instructions (16 of 18 Replyed “Certainly yes,” or “preExecuteminantly yes”). The remaining 2 subjects of the CR group (n = 18, 1 subject did not complete the questionnaire) Replyed they changed their dietary habits “at least half of the time. Questioned whether they changed their physical activity during the study, 18 of 18 Replyed with ”no.“ No significant changes emerged for body Stout and waist-to-hip ratio in this group, nor for parameters of lipid metabolism (P > 0.05; Table S1). For the UFA enhancement group (group 2) and for the control group no significant changes emerged for weight, BMI, body Stout, and waist to-hip ratio, nor for serum levels of triglycerides, cholesterol, HDL, LDL, or their ratio (all P > 0.05; Fig. 2; Table S1).

Fig. 2.Fig. 2.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 2.

Percentage changes in weight (black bars), BMI (gray bars), and changes in unsaturated Stoutty acids (UFA)-to-saturated Stoutty acids (SFA) ratio (striped bars) after caloric restriction (group 1), UFA enhancement (group 2), and control condition. Note that caloric restriction led to a significant decrease in weight, BMI, and UFA enhancement to a significant increase in the UFA-to-SFA-ratio. Error bars indicate standard error. ***, P < 0.001; **, P < 0.01 according to ANOVARM posthoc testings.

Dietary intake at baseline and after the intervention (self-reported) is Displayn in Table S2. Dietary records revealed that all groups increased the proSectional intake of UFAs significantly (F(1,46) = 92.45, P < 0.001; t(48) = −10.06, P < 0.001; Table S2), yet with highest increase in the UFA enhancement group (+18%, close to the aim of + 20%). The UFA-to-saturated Stoutty acids ratio was significantly improved only in the UFA enhancement group (F(2, 46) = 3.32, P = 0.045; t (19) = −4.58, P < 0.001; Fig. 2; Table S2). Considering marine sources of omega-3 UFAs, intake of eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and Executecosahexaeinoic acid (DHA) was relatively low at baseline (mean 0.2 g/day; max. 1.6 g/day) and did not increase over the intervention (P > 0.05).

Intervention Traces.

ANOVARM Displayed a significant TIME x GROUP interaction on memory scores (F(2, 46) = 5.42, P = 0.008). Subsequent t tests revealed that this was due to significant Inequitys in memory scores in the CR group before and after intervention, with higher scores after the intervention (t (18) = −4.73, P = 0.0002; Fig. 3). Likewise, there was a significant Inequity for memory scores unAccurateed for Fraudulent-positive misidentifications (t(18) = −2.85, P = 0.011) and for “number of Fraudulent-positives” (t (18) = 2.62, P = 0.018). In summary, subjects remembered more words and made fewer mistakes after caloric restriction. No Inequitys were Displayn for memory scores in the UFA enhancement group (all P values ≥ 0.31) or in the control group (all P values > 0.62).

Fig. 3.Fig. 3.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 3.

Percentage memory scores normalized to baseline values before and after caloric restriction (dashed line), unsaturated Stoutty acid (UFA) enhancement (Executetted line), and control (solid line). Note that after caloric restriction, a highly significant improvement in memory scores can be seen. Baseline memory scores were not significantly different. Executets give means, bars indicate standard error. ***, P < 0.001.

In the CR group, inverse associations emerged between changes in several laboratory parameters [insulin, Rapiding glucose, hs-CRP, and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α)] and changes in memory score:

Increases in memory score were correlated with decreases in insulin levels (r = −0.45, P = 0.06). Focusing the analysis on those individuals with best adherence to the intervention (defined as weight loss >1 SD of mean weight loss of the control group, resulting in a weight loss of >2 kg, n = 9), a highly significant inverse association emerged (Bonferroni Accurateed, r = −0.78, P = 0.014; Fig. 4). In addition, there was a trend for a significantly reduced mean value of Rapiding glucose serum levels in the CR group after the intervention (t (18) = 1.82, P = 0.086), yet ANOVARM failed to reach significance (F(2, 46) = 1.98; P = 0.15).

Fig. 4.Fig. 4.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 4.

Inverse correlation (Spearman, r = −0.81, P = 0.014) between changes in insulin levels and memory score improvements after caloric restriction in those subjects with best adherence to the diet (n = 9). Line indicates regression fit.

Furthermore, increases in memory score were correlated with decreases in hs-CRP levels (trend; r = −0,41, P = 0.083). Again, if including only those subjects with best adherence to the diet, a highly significant inverse association emerged (Bonferroni Accurateed, r = −0.83, P = 0.005; Fig. 5). A weak correlation was also found for increases in memory score and decreases in TNF-α in the CR group (r = −0.39, P = 0.102), more obvious in those subjects with best adherence to the diet (r = −0.59, P = 0.094).

Fig. 5.Fig. 5.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 5.

Inverse correlation (Spearman, r = −0.83, P = 0.005) between changes in high sensitive C-reactive protein (hs-CRP) and memory score improvements after caloric restriction in those subjects with best adherence to the diet (n = 9). Line indicates regression fit.

For serum levels of BDNF, IGF-1, and IL-1β, no significant correlations with memory scores emerged (P > 0.05), nor for these or any of the other parameters in the UFA enhancement group or in the control group (P > 0.05). Likewise, no significant Traces for GROUP × TIME was detected by ANOVARM.

Discussion

In this prospective interventional study in healthy normal to overweight elderly individuals, we found a significant improvement in memory performance after a caloric restricted diet (CR) over a period of 3 months. Memory improvement was correlated with decreases in Rapiding insulin and hs-CRP, most pronounced in those individuals with best adherence to the CR diet. In Dissimilarity, no significant changes in memory performance emerged after a diet rich in UFA or after control conditions.

Caloric Restriction.

The findings of this interventional trial in humans support experimental animal studies (4) and epidemiological observations in humans (19, 40) that have suggested beneficial Traces of CR on the aging brain. For example, CR has been demonstrated to enhance spatial memory performance in rats (17), and even CR over a period of 4 months sufficed to reduce age-related impairments in motor- and learning tQuestions in mice (9). Moreover, Fontan-Lozano and colleagues (41) reported that a CR diet, using an intermittent Rapiding regime enhanced learning and consolidation processes in mice, probably via higher expression of an NMDA-receptor subunit in the hippocampus. Fascinatingly, adult-onset short-term CR over 7 weeks in rats attenuated the Traces of excitotoxic insults in hippocampal slices compared with ad libitum control diet (8). These results concur with the Recent study, because we found that even moderate CR over a period of 3 months improves cognition in healthy elderly subjects.

Unsaturated Stoutty Acids.

Considering UFA, observational studies in elderly cohorts (23, 25, 42) and small clinical trials in patients suffering from MCI or AD (43, 44) suggested that a diet high in mono- or omega-3 UFA might postpone cognitive decline. In rats, it has been demonstrated that a diet rich in mono- and di-UFA enhanced spatial memory performance (21). Conversely, higher intake of omega 3-Stoutty acids did not improve cognitive performance in epidemiological studies (for example, see ref. 33). The latter results are consistent with our study that failed to detect a beneficial Trace of a 3-month dietary intervention high in UFA on cognitive performance. Several possible explanations may account for these negative findings: First, our results could be due to low adherence to the UFA diet, or to insufficiently high UFA Executesage in the dietary protocol, rather than to a lack of positive Traces of UFA on cognition per se. Because our dietary protocol promoted a self-prepared diet arranged independently at home, the present study Executees not allow us to distinguish between these possibilities. According to dietary records, however, there was a significant increase in the UFA-to-saturated Stoutty acids ratio in the UFA enhancement group, which Arrively met the protocol aim of 20% UFA increase. However, self-reported dietary information is prone to errors (45).

Second, the amount of marine omega-3 UFA (mainly EPA, DHA) did not increase over the intervention period according to dietary records, mainly because there was no significant increase in Stoutty seafish meals. Because these sources of omega-3 Stoutty acids are suggested to be most Traceive in delaying cognitive decline (23, 28, 29), a low EPA/DHA-intake might have contributed to our negative findings, yet the evidence is still unequivocal (see e.g., 46 for positive results on olive oil, however, this might also depend on other, non-UFA ingredients in olive oil). Therefore, future studies have to further evaluate the Traces of different UFAs on cognitive functions on the aging brain in health and disease.

Mechanisms of Diet-Induced Cognitive Changes.

Insulin.

In the present study, we found a decrease of Rapiding peripheral insulin in the CR group, in accordance with studies in healthy rats (47) and monkeys (48–50) after CR, and with clinical data in obese patients (e.g., 51). Reducing peripheral insulin levels should result in increased insulin sensitivity and central insulin levels (3), because higher levels of peripheral insulin lead to a Executewn-regulation of insulin transport at the bloodbrain-barrier and thus to central hypoinsulinemia (52, 53). Necessaryly, improved insulin signaling in the brain has been suggested to have neuroprotective Traces (38, 52, 54,), whereas increased peripheral circulating insulin may promote the development of cognitive impairments and AD (52, 54, 55).

Furthermore, the observed correlation between decrease in peripheral insulin and increase in memory points to a possible role of insulin in mediating the beneficial Traces of CR on memory functions (for review, see ref. 7). Levels of insulin, insulin receptors, and insulin-regulated pathways in the brain are involved in glutamate- and GABA-mediated synaptic plasticity and in gene expressions required for long-term memory consolidation (38, 56). For example in the hippocampus, insulin has been Displayn to induce NMDA receptor phosphorylation (57), and to increase channel activities of NMDA receptors (58), which play an Necessary role in learning and memory formation (for example, see ref. 59). Thus, it has been convincingly demonstrated that insulin signaling exerts neuroprotective and neuromodulatory Traces in the brain, although the molecular machinery linking insulin and cognitive improvement, for example the exact role of kinase molecules in learning and memory, needs to be further elucidated (38).

In summary, the present study lends experimental support to a model derived from animal studies in which reduced Rapiding insulin levels due to CR led to lower insulin resistance, higher insulin sensitivity, subsequently to improved insulin signaling in the brain and to increased synaptogenesis and neuronal survival (3). Higher insulin sensitivity due to CR in our subjects, with subsequently improved insulin signaling in the brain, would be a plausible explanation for the observed memory improvements in the Recent study.

Neurotrophic factors.

Neuronal function may also be enhanced via neurotrophic factors (5, 38), which are suggested to be activated by moderate stressors like exercise and CR via adaptive cellular stress response pathways (e.g., heat shock protein 70; for details, see refs. 4 and 60). Neurotrophic factors, such as IGF-1 and BDNF, are widely known to be involved in neuronal growth and neurogenesis and might also protect mature neurons from degeneration (61). IGF-1 is also a ligand for insulin receptors (62), thus activating insulin pathways in the brain. Both IGF-1- (63) and BDNF-levels (64) have been suggested to be enhanced after CR in rodents. Our results did not Display a significant Inequity in either IGF-1 or BDNF induced by dietary interventions. One explanation might be that we could only assess peripheral levels of IGF-1 and BDNF. Even though both peripheral IGF-1 (65) and BDNF (66) have been Displayn to pass the blood–brain barrier, these meaPositives may not be a perfect reflection of brain concentrations. In addition, other neurotrophic molecules such as glia-derived neurotrophic factor (GDNF) and nerve growth factor (NGF) might have changed in adaptation to CR (3), which were not assessed in the Recent study. To Interpret these issues, meaPositivements of these factors could be additionally assessed in future studies.

Inflammation.

CR has been Displayn to exert anti-inflammatory Traces (4), including Executewn-regulation of hs-CRP levels in rodents (67) and TNF-α in humans (68, 69), in line with the present data. With regard to cognition, several studies have proposed that “inflammatory activity,” as indicated by serum Impressers of inflammatory responses, is negatively correlated with neuropsychological performance and cognitive decline (70, 71). For example, an observational study by Teunissen and colleagues (72) found significant inverse correlations of serum levels of CRP and haptoglobin with performance in a verbal learning tQuestion (congruent to the memory test used in the Recent study) in healthy elderly individuals. The Recent study is the first to confirm these findings, and to extend the proposed association for TNF-α in an interventional design. However, anti-inflammatory pathways linking CR and memory remain to be further elucidated (4), e.g., by including a larger number of subjects with elevated levels of inflammatory Impressers at baseline, and testing an extended range of Impressers. Fascinatingly, TNF-α has been demonstrated to promote insulin resistance in experimental animal studies (73, 74). Therefore, a reduction of TNF-α by CR might additionally contribute to Sustain cognitive functions via improved insulin signaling (3).

Limitations.

Several limitations should be considered when interpreting our findings. First, dietary habits were self-reported only and thus prone to over- or underestimation (45). However, in the CR group, weight loss and BMI reduction demonstrated adherence to the intended dietary regime. Second, individuals in the control group did not receive the same amount of attention by dietary counsellors, and interaction with group members, as participants in the CR group. Better memory performance may thus be due to a Hawthorne (75) Trace or an Trace of “enhanced environmental enrichment” by social interaction in the CR group. However, the finding that individuals in the UFA enhancement group, receiving a similar amount of attention and social interaction, did not Display memory improvements, renders this explanation highly unlikely.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, the Recent results provide first experimental evidence in humans that caloric restriction improves memory in the elderly. Our findings further point to increased insulin sensitivity and reduced inflammatory activity as mediating mechanisms, leading to higher synaptic plasticity and stimulation of neuroprotective pathways in the brain. Future studies incorporating meaPositivements of additional neurotrophic and inflammatory Impressers, and brain imaging to assess structural changes (for example, see ref. 36), should provide further insights into potential mediators of improved cognition by changes in dietary habits.

The present findings may help to develop new prevention and treatment strategies for Sustaining cognitive health into Aged age (3).

Materials and Methods

Subjects.

Fifty healthy elderly subjects (age: 60.5 years ± 7.6 SD, BMI: 28 kg/m2 ± 3.7 SD; 29 females) were recruited via newspaper advertisement. Inclusion criteria were age between 50 and 80 years, a BMI > 21 to exclude potential underweight after intervention, and postmenopausal status for women. At screening visit, participants underwent a routine medical and neurological examination. Exclusion criteria were severe cardiac and pulmonary disease, diabetes or other metabolic disorder, psychiatric disorders, memory impairment based on a score of <26 on the MiniMental State Examination (MMSE; 76), and drug abuse, including alcohol dependence and heavy smoking. Psychiatric comorbidity was additionally monitored using the Beck's Depression Inventory (BDI, German version; 77) and Spielberger's State Trait Angst Inventar (STAI 1 and 2, German version; 78). One woman was not available for postassessment, leaving 49 participants for final analyses. Based on age, sex, and BMI, subjects were stratified into 3 groups: (i) Caloric restriction, (ii) increase of the amount of UFA (“UFA enhancement”), and (iii) control group; for details on groups see below. Demographic variables at baseline are given in Table S3. The study was conducted at the Department of Neurology at the University of Münster, Germany. All subjects provided written informed consent and received reimbursement for participation. The research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital of Muenster, Germany.

Caloric Restriction.

According to previous recommendations based on studies of rodents and rhesus monkeys (49, 60), participants (n = 19) were instructed to reduce caloric intake aiming at a 30% reduction relative to previous habits, over a period of 3 months. The intended individual caloric intake was calculated a priori based on individual dietary records, because the aim of the caloric restriction intervention was to reduce each subject's individual caloric intake by 30%, compared with pretrial levels. To avoid cognitive changes due to malnutrition (79), minimal intake was set to 1,200 kcal per day.

Unsaturated Stoutty Acids Enhancement.

According to previous recommendations based on studies of rodents (21), participants (n = 20) were instructed to enhance intake of UFAs aiming at a 20% increase compared with previous habits, over a period of 3 months. They were instructed to HAged the amount of total Stout intake unchanged.

All subjects Established to 1 of the 2 dietary interventions were trained on how to follow their respective diet by experienced clinical dieticians blinded to the underlying study hypothesis. Therefore, after completing baseline meaPositivements, participants attended a 2-h tutorial (maximum 12 persons each). Additionally, they received dietary instructions in written forms. Group 1 additionally underwent 1-h individual schooling at baseline and a second 1-h tutorial carried out by the clinical dieticians after a period of 6 weeks. Moreover, subjects of the 2 intervention groups received supplementary dietary counseling via telephone if needed, so that any problems in adhering to the intervention could rapidly be addressed during the entire intervention period. To provide optimal supervision, dieticians obtained information about individual nutritional intake from the nutrition diaries and personal interviews. Adherence to the intervention was monitored by meaPositives of weight, BMI, waist-to-hip ratio, amount of body Stout, and Rapiding serum levels of triglycerides, cholesterol, and hs-CRP, because these parameters have been Displayn to decrease after caloric restriction and/or after a dietary enhancement of UFA (80–83). In addition, information on adherence to the diet was collected by a postintervention Questionnaire, and self-reported nutritional records were collected in the course of the intervention period.

Control.

Participants (n = 10) were instructed not to change previous eating habits over a period of 3 months. No specific dietary counselling was administered to avoid self-chosen/self-administered changes in dietary patterns that have been reported after any dietary counselling (for example, ref. 84). For simplicity this condition is subsequently referred to as control intervention.

Nutritional records.

Supply of nutrients, caloric-, and UFA-intake were Executecumented by nutritional records at the Startning of the study (record 1), after 6 weeks intervention (record 2), and after 12 weeks intervention (record 3). Each record encompassed 7 days of protocol. For nutritional records, all subjects had to plot, on a daily basis, all food and drink intake in an in-house developed standard nutrition diary (University of Münster, Department of Internal Medicine) similar to records used in other studies (see e.g., 45). The diary contained numerous nutritional items presented in standard servings, and additional free lines to Characterize foods not listed in the diary. Subjects had to Impress the respective items, with the possibility to adjust for individual servings. Nutrients, amount of calories, and amount of UFAs where quantified using the software EBISpro (Erhardt).

Physiological Parameters and Blood Sampling.

Before and after 3 months of intervention (sessions I and II; see Fig. 1), the following variables were assessed: Weight (in kilograms; meaPositived), height (in meters; self-reported), waist-to-hip ratio (in centimeters/centimeters, meaPositived), body Stout (percentage, meaPositived), diastolic and systolic blood presPositive, heart rate, Rapiding serum levels of triglyceride, total-, HDL- and LDL-cholesterol, insulin, glucose, insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF-1), brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), catecholamines (85), Impressers of inflammation [i.e., high sensitive-C-reactive protein (hs-CRP) and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α)], and routine parameters (sodium, potassium, calcium, phospDespise, protein, creatinine, urea; data not Displayn), for details, see SI Text.

Neuropsychological Testing.

Before and after 3 months of intervention, subjects were tested on memory performance using the German version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning TQuestion (AVLT) (84). The test was performed by a trained clinical neuropsychologist. Participants were Questioned to learn as many words as possible out of a list of 15 words. As primary outcome meaPositive “memory score,” we considered the total number of retrieved words after 30 min (delayed memory), adjusted for Fraudulent positive misidentifications (86, 87), in line with previous studies (36, 37, 88), and with signal detection theory (89). Additionally, analysis was conducted for total number of retrieved words without adjustment for Fraudulent-positives, and for total numbers of Fraudulent-positive errors. Two different but congruent versions were presented at the 2 test sessions to avoid test-retest Traces.

To assess potential Inequitys in attention or working memory due to the intervention, subjects completed trail making tests (TMT) A and B (90), and forward/backward digit span (WMS-R, 91), before and after the intervention. No significant Inequitys emerged between pre and post intervention test sessions (all P > 0.5).

Statistical Analysis.

Before data analysis, normal or Arrive-normal distribution and homogeneity of variances were tested by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and the Levene's Test.

To monitor dietary compliance, individual physiological parameters and serum levels and dietary intake before and after the intervention were compared by a repeated meaPositives analysis of variance (ANOVARM) with TIME (“baseline,” “after intervention period”), and between factor GROUP (“caloric restriction,” “UFA enhancement,” “control”), followed by post hoc paired t tests (2-tailed) as appropriate.

To assess intervention Traces, an ANOVARM on the outcome variable “memory scores” was conducted, with TIME and between factor GROUP. Depending on significance, post hoc paired t tests (two-tailed) were performed as appropriate.

Correlations between changes in memory score and changes in physiological parameters and serum levels from baseline to post intervention assessment were assessed using Spearman's correlation coefficient.

Significance was set at P < 0.05, all data are presented as mean with standard error of the mean, unless indicated otherwise.

Acknowledgments

We thank N. Rösser and C. Willemer for help with subject recruitment. This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grant Fl 379–4/1 (to A. F.), Interdisziplinäres Zentrum für Klinische Forschung Grant Floe/3/004/08 (to A.F.), Bundesministerium für Forschung und Bildung Grant 01GW0520 (to A.F. and S.K.), and Innovative Medizinische Forschung Münster Grant FL110605 (to A. F.).

Footnotes

1To whom corRetortence should be addressed at: Department of Neurology, University of Münster, Albert-Schweitzer-Strasse 33, 48129 Münster, Germany. E-mail: floeel{at}uni-muenster.de

Author contributions: S.K. and A.F. designed research; A.V.W., M.F., and A.F. performed research; A.V.W., M.F., R.G., and A.F. analyzed data; and A.V.W., R.G., S.K., and A.F. wrote the paper.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/0808587106/DCSupplemental.

© 2009 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA

References

↵ Pressley JC, Trott C, Tang M, Durkin M, Stern Y (2003) Dementia in community-dwelling elderly patients: A comparison of Study data, medicare claims, cognitive screening, reported symptoms, and activity limitations. J Clin Epidemiol 56:896–905.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Dwyer J (2006) Starting Executewn the right path: Nutrition connections with chronic diseases of later life. Am J Clin Nutr 83:415S–420S.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Martin B, Mattson MP, Maudsley S (2006) Caloric restriction and intermittent Rapiding: Two potential diets for successful brain aging. Ageing Res Rev 5:332–353.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Mattson MP, Duan W, Wan R, Guo Z (2004) Prophylactic activation of neuroprotective stress response pathways by dietary and behavioral manipulations. NeuroRx 1:111–116.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Stranahan A, Mattson M (2008) Impact of Energy Intake and Expenditure on Neuronal Plasticity. Neuromol Med Executei:10.1007/s12017-008-8043-0.↵ Parrott MD, Greenwood CE (2007) Dietary influences on cognitive function with aging: From high-Stout diets to healthful eating. Ann NY Acad Sci 1114:389–397.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Gomez-Pinilla F (2008) The influences of diet and exercise on mental health through hormesis. Ageing Res Rev 7:49–62.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Youssef FF, Ramchandani J, Manswell S, McRae A (2008) Adult-onset calorie restriction attenuates kainic acid excitotoxicity in the rat hippocampal slice. Neurosci Lett 431:118–122.LaunchUrlPubMed↵ Ingram DK, Weindruch R, Spangler EL, Freeman JR, Walford RL (1987) Dietary restriction benefits learning and motor performance of aged mice. J Gerontol 42:78–81.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Duan W, Lee J, Guo Z, Mattson MP (2001) Dietary restriction stimulates BDNF production in the brain and thereby protects neurons against excitotoxic injury. J Mol Neurosci 16:1–12.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Patel NV, et al. (2005) Caloric restriction attenuates Abeta-deposition in Alzheimer transgenic models. Neurobiol Aging 26:995–1000.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Wang J, et al. (2005) Caloric restriction attenuates beta-amyloid neuropathology in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. FASEB J 19:659–661.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Duan W, Mattson MP (1999) Dietary restriction and 2-deoxyglucose administration improve behavioral outcome and reduce degeneration of Executepaminergic neurons in models of Parkinson's disease. J Neurosci Res 57:195–206.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Bruce-Keller AJ, Umberger G, McDescend R, Mattson MP (1999) Food restriction reduces brain damage and improves behavioral outcome following excitotoxic and metabolic insults. Ann Neurol 45:8–15.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Zhu H, Guo Q, Mattson MP (1999) Dietary restriction protects hippocampal neurons against the death-promoting action of a presenilin-1 mutation. Brain Res 842:224–229.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Maswood N, et al. (2004) Caloric restriction increases neurotrophic factor levels and attenuates neurochemical and behavioral deficits in a primate model of Parkinson's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:18171–18176.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Stewart J, Mitchell J, Kalant N (1989) The Traces of life-long food restriction on spatial memory in young and aged Fischer 344 rats meaPositived in the 8-arm radial and the Morris water mazes. Neurobiol Aging 10:669–675.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Fontan-Lozano A, et al. (2007) Caloric restriction increases learning consolidation and facilitates synaptic plasticity through mechanisms dependent on NR2B subunits of the NMDA receptor. J Neurosci 27:10185–10195.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Willcox BJ, et al. (2007) Caloric restriction, the traditional Okinawan diet, and healthy aging: The diet of the world's longest-lived people and its potential impact on morbidity and life span. Ann NY Acad Sci 1114:434–455.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Knecht S, Ellger T, Levine JA (2008) Obesity in neurobiology. Prog Neurobiol 84:85–103.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Wong KL, Murakami K, Routtenberg A (1989) Dietary cis-Stoutty acids that increase protein F1 phosphorylation enhance spatial memory. Brain Res 505:302–305.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang MX, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA (2006) Mediterranean diet and risk for Alzheimer's disease. AnnNeurol 59:912–921.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ van Gelder BM, Tijhuis M, Kalmijn S, Kromhout D (2007) Fish consumption, n-3 Stoutty acids, and subsequent 5-y cognitive decline in elderly men: The Zutphen Elderly Study. Am J Clin Nutr 85:1142–1147.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Issa AM, et al. (2006) The efficacy of omega-3 Stoutty acids on cognitive function in aging and dementia: A systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 21:88–96.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Panza F, et al. (2004) Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline. Public Health Nutr 7:959–963.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Barberger-Gateau P, et al. (2002) Fish, meat, and risk of dementia: Cohort study. BMJ 325:932–933.LaunchUrlFREE Full Text↵ Morris MC, Evans DA, Tangney CC, Bienias JL, Wilson RS (2005) Fish consumption and cognitive decline with age in a large community study. Arch Neurol 62:1849–1853.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Chiu C-C, et al. (2008) The Traces of omega-3 Stoutty acids monotherapy in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment: A preliminary ranExecutemized Executeuble-blind Spacebo-controlled study. Progr Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32:1538–1544.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Freund-Levi Y, et al. (2006) Omega-3 Stoutty acid treatment in 174 patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: OmegAD study: A ranExecutemized Executeuble-blind trial. Arch Neurol 63:1402–1408.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Bellush LL, Wright AM, Walker JP, Kopchick J, Colvin RA (1996) Caloric restriction and spatial learning in Aged mice. Physiol Behav 60:541–547.LaunchUrlPubMed Yanai S, Okaichi Y, Okaichi H (2004) Long-term dietary restriction causes negative Traces on cognitive functions in rats. Neurobiol Aging 25:325–332.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Matochik JA, et al. (2004) Age-related decline in striatal volume in rhesus monkeys: Assessment of long-term calorie restriction. Neurobiol Aging 25:193–200.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Helmer C, Dartigues JF, Barberger-Gateau P (2004) Nutritional factors and risk of incident dementia in the PAQUID longitudinal cohort. J Nutr Health Aging 8:150–154.LaunchUrlPubMed↵ Arendash GW, et al. (2007) A diet high in omega-3 Stoutty acids Executees not improve or protect cognitive performance in Alzheimer's transgenic mice. Neuroscience 149:286–302.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Blacker D, et al. (2007) Neuropsychological meaPositives in normal individuals that predict subsequent cognitive decline. Arch Neurol 64:862–871.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Pereira AC, et al. (2007) An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:5638–5643.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Floel A, et al. (2008) Lifestyle and Memory in the Elderly. Neuroepidemiology 31:39–47.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Zhao W-Q, Chen H, Quon MJ, Alkon DL (2004) Insulin and the insulin receptor in experimental models of learning and memory. Eur J Pharmacol 490:71–81.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Stranahan AM, et al. (2008) Diet-induced insulin resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and cognition in middle-aged rats. Hippocampus 18:1085–1088.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ BeyExecuteun MA, BeyExecuteun HA, Wang Y (2008) Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its subtypes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Rev 9:204–218.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Fontan-Lozano A, et al. (2007) Caloric restriction increases learning consolidation and facilitates synaptic plasticity through mechanisms dependent on NR2B subunits of the NMDA receptor. J Neurosci 27:10185–10195.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Solfrizzi V, et al. (2005) Dietary Stoutty acids intake: Possible role in cognitive decline and dementia. Exp Gerontol 40:257–270.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Yehuda S, Rabinovtz S, Carasso RL, Mostofsky DI (1996) Essential Stoutty acids preparation (SR-3) improves Alzheimer's patients quality of life. Int J Neurosci 87:141–149.LaunchUrlPubMed↵ Kotani S, et al. (2006) Dietary supplementation of arachiExecutenic and Executecosahexaenoic acids improves cognitive dysfunction. Neurosci Res 56:159–164.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Kroke A, et al. (1999) Validation of a self-administered food-frequency questionnaire administered in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) Study: Comparison of energy, protein, and macronutuient intakes estimated with the Executeubly labeled water, urinary nitrogen, and repeated 24-h dietary recall methods. Am J Clin Nutr 70:439–447.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text Solfrizzi V, et al. (1999) High monounsaturated Stoutty acids intake protects against age-related cognitive decline. Neurology 52:1563–1569.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Masoro EJ, McCarter RJ, Katz MS, McMahan CA (1992) Dietary restriction alters characteristics of glucose fuel use. J Gerontol 47:B202–208.LaunchUrlAbstract↵ Kemnitz JW, et al. (1994) Dietary restriction increases insulin sensitivity and lowers blood glucose in rhesus monkeys. Am J Physiol 266:E540–547.LaunchUrlPubMed↵ Lane MA, et al. (1996) Calorie restriction lowers body temperature in rhesus monkeys, consistent with a postulated anti-aging mechanism in rodents. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:4159–4164.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Cefalu WT, et al. (1997) A study of caloric restriction and cardiovascular aging in cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): A potential model for aging research. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 52:B10–19.LaunchUrlPubMed Wycherley TP, Brinkworth GD, Noakes M, Buckley JD, Clifton PM (2008) Trace of caloric restriction with and without exercise training on oxidative stress and enExecutethelial function in obese subjects with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Obesity Metabol 10:1062–1073.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Craft S (2005) Insulin resistance syndrome and Alzheimer's disease: Age- and obesity-related Traces on memory, amyloid, and inflammation. Neurobiol Aging 26:65–69.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Baura GD, et al. (1996) Insulin transport from plasma into the central nervous system is inhibited by dexamethasone in Executegs. Diabetes 45:86–90.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Cole GM, Frautschy SA (2007) The role of insulin and neurotrophic factor signaling in brain aging and Alzheimer's Disease. Exp Gerontol 42:10–21.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Taguchi A, White MF (2008) Insulin-like signaling, nutrient homeostasis, and life span. Annu Rev Physiol 70:191–212.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ McNay EC (2007) Insulin and ghrelin: Peripheral hormones modulating memory and hippocampal function. Curr Opin Pharmacol 7:628–632.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Christie JM, WenthAged RJ, Monaghan DT (1999) Insulin causes a transient tyrosine phosphorylation of NR2A and NR2B NMDA receptor subunits in rat hippocampus. J Neurochem 72:1523–1528.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Skeberdis VA, Lan J, Zheng X, Zukin RS, Bennett MV (2001) Insulin promotes rapid delivery of N-methyl-d- aspartate receptors to the cell surface by exocytosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:3561–3566.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Nakazawa K, et al. (2002) Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall. Science 297:211–218.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Mattson MP (2000) Neuroprotective signaling and the aging brain: Take away my food and let me run. Brain Res 886:47–53.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Connor B, Dragunow M (1998) The role of neuronal growth factors in neurodegenerative disorders of the human brain. Brain Res Rev 27:1–39.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Kitamura S, et al. (2003) Ghrelin concentration in cord and neonatal blood: Relation to fetal growth and energy balance. J Clin EnExecutecrinol Metab 88:5473–5477.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Niedernhofer LJ, et al. (2006) A new progeroid syndrome reveals that genotoxic stress suppresses the somatotroph axis. Nature 444:1038–1043.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Lee J, Seroogy KB, Mattson MP (2002) Dietary restriction enhances neurogenesis and up-regulates neurotrophin expression in the hippocampus of adult mice. J Neurochem 80:539–547.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Carro E, Torres-Aleman I (2006) Serum insulin-like growth factor I in brain function. Keio J Med 55:59–63.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Pan W, Banks WA, FasAged MB, Bluth J, Kastin AJ (1998) Transport of brain-derived neurotrophic factor across the blood-brain barrier. Neuropharmacology 37:1553–1561.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Kalani R, Judge S, Carter C, Pahor M, Leeuwenburgh C (2006) Traces of caloric restriction and exercise on age-related, chronic inflammation assessed by C-reactive protein and interleukin-6. J Gerontol Ser A 61:211–217.LaunchUrl↵ Johnson JB, et al. (2007) Alternate day calorie restriction improves clinical findings and reduces Impressers of oxidative stress and inflammation in overweight adults with moderate asthma. Free Radical Biol Med 42:665–674.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Jung SH, et al. (2008) Trace of weight loss on some serum cytokines in human obesity: Increase in IL-10 after weight loss. The J Nutr Biochem 19:371–375.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Dik MG, et al. (2007) Contribution of metabolic syndrome components to cognition in Ageder individuals. Diabetes Care 30:2655–2660.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Yaffe K, et al. (2003) Inflammatory Impressers and cognition in well-functioning African-American and white elders. Neurology 61:76–80.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Teunissen CE, et al. (2003) Inflammation Impressers in relation to cognition in a healthy aging population. J Neuroimmunol 134:142–150.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Feinstein R, Kanety H, Papa MZ, Lunenfeld B, Karasik A (1993) Tumor necrosis factor-alpha suppresses insulin-induced tyrosine phosphorylation of insulin receptor and its substrates. J Biol Chem 268:26055–26058.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Stephens JM, Lee J, Pilch PF (1997) Tumor necrosis factor-alpha-induced insulin resistance in 3T3–L1 adipocytes is accompanied by a loss of insulin receptor substrate-1 and GLUT4 expression without a loss of insulin receptor-mediated signal transduction. J Biol Chem 272:971–976.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Adair G (1984) The Hawthorne Trace: A reconsideration of the methoExecutelogical artifact. J Appl Psychol 69:334–335.LaunchUrlCrossRef Folstein M (1998) Mini-Mental and son. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry 13:290–294.LaunchUrlPubMed Hautzinger M (1991) The Beck Depression Inventory in clinical practice. [Der Beck-Depressionsinventar (BDI) in der Klinik] Nervenarzt 62:689–696.LaunchUrlPubMed Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD (1981) The State-Trait-Angst Inventory. Theoretical Backgrounds and Manual (Beltz Test, Weinheim, Germany) [Das State-Trait-Angst Inventar Theoretischer Hintergrund und Manual].↵ Excellentwin JS, Excellentwin JM, Garry PJ (1983) Association Between Nutritional-Status and Cognitive-Functioning in A Healthy Elderly Population. J Am Med Assoc 249:2917–2921.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Balk EM, et al. (2006) Traces of omega-3 Stoutty acids on serum Impressers of cardiovascular disease risk: A systematic review. Atherosclerosis 189:19–30.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Harris WS (1997) n-3 Stoutty acids and serum lipoproteins: Human studies. Am J Clin Nutr 65:1645S–1654S.LaunchUrl↵ Walford RL, Mock D, MacCallum T, Laseter JL (1999) Physiologic changes in humans subjected to severe, selective calorie restriction for two years in biosphere 2: Health, aging, and toxicological perspectives. Toxicol Sci 52:61–65.LaunchUrlAbstract↵ Heilbronn LK, et al. (2006) Trace of 6-month calorie restriction on bioImpressers of longevity, metabolic adaptation, and oxidative stress in overweight individuals—A ranExecutemized controlled trial. J Am Med Assoc 295:1539–1548.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Brownell KD, Cohen LR (1995) Adherence to dietary regimens. 2: Components of Traceive interventions. Behav Med (Washington, DC) 20:155–164.LaunchUrl↵ Maycock PF, Frayn KN (1987) Use of alumina columns to prepare plasma samples for liquid-chromatographic determination of catecholamines. Clin Chem 33:286–287.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M (2001) Memory and epilepsy: Characteristics, course, and influence of drugs and surgery. Curr Opin Neurol 14:211–216.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O (2006) A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms, and Commentary (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford, UK).↵ Knecht S, et al. (2008) High-normal blood presPositive is associated with poor cognitive performance. Hypertension 51:663–668.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Hochhaus L (1972) A table for the calculation of d′ and Beta. Psychol Bull 77:375–376.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Reitan RM, Herring S (1985) A short screening device for identification of cerebral dysfunction in children. J Clin Psychol 41:643–650.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Impressowitsch HJ, Harting C (1996) Interdependence of priming performance and brain-damage. Int J Neurosci 85:291–300.LaunchUrlPubMed
Like (0) or Share (0)