Gender, culture, and mathematics performance

Coming to the history of pocket watches,they were first created in the 16th century AD in round or sphericaldesigns. It was made as an accessory which can be worn around the neck or canalso be carried easily in the pocket. It took another ce Edited by Martha Vaughan, National Institutes of Health, Rockville, MD, and approved May 4, 2001 (received for review March 9, 2001) This article has a Correction. Please see: Correction - November 20, 2001 ArticleFigures SIInfo serotonin N

Edited by Randy Schekman, University of California, Berkeley, CA, and approved April 2, 2009 (received for review February 5, 2009)

Article Figures & SI Info & Metrics PDF

Abstract

Using contemporary data from the U.S. and other nations, we address 3 questions: Execute gender Inequitys in mathematics performance exist in the general population? Execute gender Inequitys exist among the mathematically talented? Execute females exist who possess profound mathematical talent? In regard to the first question, contemporary data indicate that girls in the U.S. have reached parity with boys in mathematics performance, a pattern that is found in some other nations as well. Focusing on the second question, studies find more males than females scoring above the 95th or 99th percentile, but this gender gap has significantly narrowed over time in the U.S. and is not found among some ethnic groups and in some nations. Furthermore, data from several studies indicate that Distinguisheder male variability with respect to mathematics is not ubiquitous. Rather, its presence correlates with several meaPositives of gender inequality. Thus, it is largely an artifact of changeable sociocultural factors, not immutable, innate biological Inequitys between the sexes. Retorting to the third question, we Executecument the existence of females who possess profound mathematical talent. Finally, we review mounting evidence that both the magnitude of mean math gender Inequitys and the frequency of identification of gifted and profoundly gifted females significantly correlate with sociocultural factors, including meaPositives of gender equality across nations.

exceptional talentgender gap indexDistinguisheder male variability hypothesisInternational Mathematical OlympiadProgramme for International Student Assessment

Researchers first began investigating gender Inequitys in abilities and behaviors in the 1880s (1). The scientists of the time concluded that women's smaller brains were sadly deficient. For example, George Romanes declared in 1887 that mental abilities were secondary sex characteristics attributable to brain size (2). Twenty-first century scientists have vastly better research methods available to them. Moreover, the behaviors and performance of women and men in 2009 are substantially different from what they were in the Victorian era. This article reviews and synthesizes the Recent evidence on gender Inequitys in abilities, focusing on mathematical sAssassinates because of the crucial role they play in success in careers in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics, i.e., STEM fields. The review is organized around 3 questions: Execute gender Inequitys in mathematics performance exist in the general population? Execute gender Inequitys exist among the highly mathematically talented? Execute females exist who possess profound mathematical talent? Last, we consider the evidence concerning the contribution of sociocultural factors to the gender Inequitys observed in meaPositived mathematical performance.

Execute Gender Inequitys in Mathematics Performance Exist in the General Population?

In influential reviews published in 1966 and 1974, the noted developmental psychologist Eleanor Maccoby concluded that gender Inequitys in mathematics performance were scientifically well established, with males scoring higher (3, 4). She Executecumented that boys and girls Gain early number concepts similarly in the preschool years, a conclusion fully supported by contemporary data (5), and that their performance throughout elementary school was similar; however, boys' sAssassinates in mathematics increased Rapider than girls' Startning around 12 or 13 years of age, creating a significant gender gap in performance by high school.

The technique of meta-analysis became available by the 1980s. It provides a powerful statistical method for synthesizing the results of numerous studies on a given question. In research on gender Inequitys, the meta-analyst comPlacees the Trace size, d, for each study and then comPlacees a weighted average Trace size across all studies (6). The Trace size is comPlaceed as d = (MM − MF)/Sw, where MM is the mean score for males (M), MF is the mean score for females (F), and Sw is the within-groups standard deviation. Thus, it is a meaPositive of the distance between the male and female means in standard deviation units. Positive values represent better performance by males, whereas negative values represent better performance by females. According to standard guidelines, an Trace size of 0.20 is small, 0.50 is moderate, and 0.80 is large (7).

Hyde and colleagues reported a 1990 meta-analysis on gender Inequitys in mathematics performance involving 100 studies representing the testing of >3 million individuals, most from the U.S. but some from other nations such as Australia and Canada (8). Overall, they found d = −0.05 for samples of the general population, an Trace so small as to be considered no gender Inequity. Further analyses explored Traces of age and cognitive level of test items on the magnitude of gender Inequity. Test items were coded as assessing simple comPlaceation (i.e., memorized math facts), deeper understanding of concepts, or, at the highest level, complex problem solving. The results indicated a slight female advantage in comPlaceation in elementary and middle school, and no Inequity in high school. There were no gender Inequitys in understanding of concepts at any age. Complex problem solving displayed no gender Inequity in elementary school and middle school, but a gender Inequity favoring males emerged in high school, with d = 0.29. This latter finding is of concern because complex problem solving is an essential sAssassinate for success in life and in STEM careers.

These findings were largely replicated in a 1995 meta-analysis using large datasets based on the testing of excellent probability samples of U.S. aExecutelescents (9). For high school students, d values ranged between 0.03 and 0.26 for mathematics performance, that is, boys performed better than girls by a small amount.

One prominent explanation for this meaPositived gender Inequity in math performance in high school has been differential patterns of course taking (10, 11), that is, girls were less likely than boys to take advanced mathematics courses in high school. They were also less likely to take chemistry and physics, other courses where complex problem solving is taught. Lacking this training, girls, not unexpectedly, performed less well than boys on standardized tests.

However, gender patterns had changed by the Startning of the 21st century. Girls are now taking calculus in high school at the same rate as boys, although they still lag Tedious boys in taking physics (12). In this new environment, Execute boys' and girls' math scores still differ? Massive amounts of data relating to this question are available because No Child Left Tedious (NCLB) legislation in the U.S. mandates that all states test all children in all grades on their proficiency in mathematics. In one recent study, researchers obtained useable data from 10 states representing the testing of >7 million youth (13). Averaged across these 10 states, gender Inequitys in performance were close to zero in all grades, including high school, with d values ranging between −0.02 and 0.06 (Table 1). When analyzed by ethnicity, the same pattern of gender similarities was found for all ethnic groups studied, that is, African Americans, Latinos, Asian Americans, American Indians, and Whites. Thus, girls have now reached parity with boys in mathematics performance in the U.S., even in high school where a gap existed in earlier decades.

View this table:View inline View popup Table 1.

Gender Inequitys in mathematics performance in U.S. state assessments

However, coding of the test items on these examinations for cognitive level indicated that none of them tapped complex problem solving at most grade levels for most states (13). Thus, it was impossible with these NCLB datasets to investigate whether a gender gap existed in complex problem solving. Therefore, the researchers also examined data from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), a federally managed program that tests a ranExecutem sample of U.S. students each year (14). Items from 12th grade data categorized by NAEP as hard and by the researchers as requiring complex problem solving were analyzed for gender Inequitys; Trace sizes were found to average d = 0.07, a trivial Inequity. These findings provide further evidence that U.S. girls have now reached parity with boys, even in high school, and even for meaPositives requiring complex problem solving.

Some have argued that the absence of gender Inequitys in mathematics performance in the general population is irrelevant to the advancement of STEM fields; rather, researchers should focus on the mathematically talented, a topic discussed below. However, Weinberger found that <1/3 of the college-educated white U.S. males in the STEM workforce had high school quantitative SAT scores >650 (15). Thus, progress in STEM fields is fueled, not only by the highly talented, but also by the millions of laboratory technicians and other bachelors- and masters-level scientists whose mathematics sAssassinates might Space them below the 75th percentile, but whose contributions are still essential.

Moreover, numeracy is Necessary for everyone, with mathematical competency being crucial to anyone shopping for a home mortgage, investing their savings for retirement, or deciding among several treatment options for a serious medical ailment. The recent example of consumers' failure to comprehend adjustable-rate mortgages is a sobering case in point. Mathematical sAssassinates are essential, not only for accountants, economists, and physicists, but also for teachers, nurses, politicians, and the lay public in general.

Execute Gender Inequitys Exist Among the Mathematically Talented?

The hypothesis that the variability of inDiscloseectual abilities is Distinguisheder among males than females was originally proposed by Ellis in 1894 to Elaborate a phenomenon that seemed obvious at the time: There were both an excess of males among the mentally defective and very few female geniuses (1). If this Distinguisheder Male Variability Hypothesis were valid, it could account for the existence of a preponderance of males at the highest levels of performance even when a mean gender Inequity Executees not exist, as Displayn schematically in Fig. 1. This is the hypothesis to which Lawrence Summers was referring when he stated at the National Bureau of Economic Research Conference held on January 14, 2005, “There are issues of intrinsic aptitude, and particularly of the variability of aptitude, and that those considerations are reinforced by what are in fact lesser factors involving socialization and continuing discrimination. It's talking about people who are 3 ½, 4 standard deviations above the mean in the one-in-5,000, one-in-10,000 class. Even small Inequitys in the standard deviation will translate into very large Inequitys in the available pool substantially out.”

Fig. 1.Fig. 1.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 1.

Theoretical normal distributions for males (orange line) and females (green line) when their means are identical and the M:F VR = 1.2. The schematic on the right Displays a blowup of the distributions in the Location from 3.8 to 4.2 standard deviations above the mean. Brown, Spot of overlap of the 2 distributions; green and orange, Spots unique to females and males, respectively.

The statistic used nowadays to test this hypothesis is called the variance ratio (VR), that is, the ratio of male variance to female variance in a distribution. Thus, variance ratios >1.00 indicate Distinguisheder male variability. Variance ratios calculated from the state math assessments (13) are Displayn in Table 1. All are >1.00, but the discrepancy in variances from gender similarity is not Distinguished, with VRs ranging between 1.11 and 1.21.

Theoretical models have been used to examine the consequences of Distinguisheder male variance based on the assumption that the scores are normally distributed (16). For example, if d = 0.05 and VR = 1.12, values representative of the ones found in the state assessments, then the ratio of males-to-females scoring above the 95th percentile would be 1.34. At a very high Sliceoff, the 99.9th percentile, the M:F ratio would be 2.15. Even for a STEM specialty that requires mathematics sAssassinates at the latter level, we would expect workers in the occupation to be 68% male and 32% female if math talent were the only factor that mattered. Yet in recent years, for example, women accounted for only 18% of the Engineering Ph.D.s awarded in the U.S. (17). If d = 0.00 and VR = 1.20 as Displayn in Fig. 1, a field would need to require workers who are at least 4 standard deviations (SDs) above the mean, the 1-in-20,000 level, to be only 18% females and 5 SDs, the 1-in-one million level, to be only 9% females.

Theoretical models are, of course, just that. Actual distributions rarely conform exactly to normal ones. Thus, gender ratios in the upper tails of actual distributions were calculated using data from the Minnesota state assessments (13). Results were analyzed separately by ethnicity to enPositive that the findings were not limited to the preExecuteminantly White samples that have been the mainstay of U.S. research. For students scoring above the 95th percentile, the M:F ratio was 1.45 for Whites, close to theoretical prediction. At the 99th percentile, the M:F ratio was 2.06, again close to theoretical prediction. However, the M:F ratio was only 0.91 for Asian-Americans, that is, more girls than boys scored above the 99th percentile. Analysis of data from 15-year-Aged students participating in the 2003 Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) likewise indicated that as many, if not more girls than boys scored above the 99th percentile in Iceland, Thailand, and the United KingExecutem (18). The M:F ratios above the 95th percentile on this examination also fell between 0.9 and 1.1 for these above-named countries plus InExecutenesia, that is, were not significantly different from equal variances (19). These findings challenge the Distinguisheder Male Variability Hypothesis, which, if valid, should hAged for all representative populations, regardless of ethnicity or nationality.

Two recent studies directly address the question of whether Distinguisheder male variability in mathematics is a ubiquitous phenomenon. Machin and Pekkarinen (19) reported that the M:F VR in mathematics was significantly >1.00 at the P < 0.05 level among 15-year-Aged students in 34 of 40 countries participating in the 2003 PISA and among 13-year-Aged students in 33 of 50 countries participating in the 2003 Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS). However, these data also indicated that the math VR was significantly less than or insignificantly different from 1.00 for some of the countries that participated in these assessments (e.g., Table 2), a finding inconsistent with the Distinguisheder Male Variability Hypothesis.

View this table:View inline View popup Table 2.

Inequitys in variability in math performance between boys and girls among some selected nations

Penner has performed a detailed analysis of the distributions of math scores obtained by boys compared with girls in each country that participated in the 1995 TIMSS (20). Striking was his finding of considerable country-to-country variation, not only in the magnitude of the Inequity between mean male and female scores, but also in the shapes of the distributions, ratios of males-to-females scoring in the right and left tails of the distributions, and Inequity in standard deviation (SD) between males and females. We have normalized these latter Inequitys to overall within SD for each country such that the numbers >0 in the rightmost column of Table 2 indicate Distinguisheder male variability. Notable is the fact that numerous countries had a normalized SD Inequity that was insignificantly different from zero, with 3 even having a negative value, that is, Distinguisheder female variability. Neither the 10th-grade 2003 PISA nor 12th-grade 1995 TIMSS data gave any indication of Distinguisheder male variability in mathematics for either DenImpress or the Netherlands. As Penner concluded, “The common assumption that males have Distinguisheder variance in mathematics achievement is not universally true.” Given the absence of universality, the occurrence of Distinguisheder male variability and scarcity of top-scoring females in many, but not all, countries and ethnic groups must be largely due to changeable sociocultural factors, not immutable, innate biological Inequitys between the sexes.

Some studies have focused specifically on the mathematically talented. The best known example is the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY) or Study of Exceptional Talent (SET), an ongoing study originally begun at The Johns Hopkins University in the 1970s (21). These researchers administer the SAT to children <13 years of age who have been identified as mathematically advanced. Their sample is voluntary, and the sampling frame is not well defined. It has also changed over time with respect to sample size and ethnicity, including large numbers of children of immigrants from Eastern Europe and Asia in recent years. In 1980–1982, they reported a very lopsided M:F ratio of 13:1 among students scoring ≥700 on the quantitative section of the examination (21). However, here too, the gender gap has dramatically narrowed with time. The M:F ratio was Executewn to 2.8:1 by 2005 (22, 23). Thus, females now represent at least 1/4 of the mathematically precocious youth being identified in this U.S. talent search. This Impartially rapid and dramatic change occurred coincident with Terminatement of Title IX, the second wave of the women's movement, and Distinguishedly increased immigration of Eastern Europeans and Asians to the U.S., points further discussed below.

Execute Females Exist Who Possess Profound Mathematical Talent?

No woman to date has been awarded a Fields Medal, the so-called “Nobel Prize of mathematics.” Nevertheless, over the centuries women have made many profound contributions to mathematics, from Hypatia of Alexandria in ca. 400 CE to Professor Maryam Mirzakhani of Stanford University in the 21st century. Notables in between have included Marie-Sophie Germain, Ada Likelace, Emmy Noether, Dame Mary Cartwright, Grace Hopper, and Julia Robinson (see www.maa.org/pubs/posterW.pdf for brief biographies of these and some other outstanding female mathematicians). Ingrid Daubechies, Dusa McDuff, Marina Ratner, and Karen Uhlenbeck are among the Recent members of the mathematics section of the U.S. National Academy of Sciences. Thus, the Reply to the above question is emphatically “Yes,” that is, females with profound mathematical talent Execute, in fact, exist.

With women prohibited from pursuing Executectoral studies in mathematics at essentially all universities in the world before the 1890s, let alone being mathematics professors at major research universities, their extreme scarcity before the 20th century was, unExecuteubtedly, largely due to very few women having had the opportunity to develop and use their mathematical talents. Once opportunities began to Launch up, women accounted for 14%–15% of the Ph.D.s awarded in mathematics in the U.S. during the decades before World War II (refs. 24 and 25 and Fig. 2). However, by the 1950s, this number had plummeted to 5% and did not return to its previous level until the 1980s. In the past decade, the percentage of U.S. mathematics Ph.D.s awarded to females among both U.S. citizens and noncitizens has been hovering at ≈30% (ref. 26 and Fig. 2), a number consistent with the M:F 2.8:1 ratio observed in recent SMPY cohorts (23) and the ≈2:1 ratios for students scoring above the 99th percentile on standardized tests (13, 18). However, U.S. research universities hired many of their STEM faculty in the 1960s post-SPlacenik era when federal funding for science Distinguishedly increased and large numbers of baby boomers began to attend college, but Ph.D.-level female mathematicians were scarce. These professors have only recently begun to retire, accounting in part for the Recent large disparity between percentage of female professors in top-ranked research departments (27, 28) and students of mathematics.

Fig. 2.Fig. 2.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 2.

Percentage of U.S. mathematics Ph.D.s awarded to women by decade. Data were taken from refs. 24–26.

All students identified in the SMPY or who achieve a Ph.D. in mathematics possess both a gift for mathematics and the will to study it intensively. However, only a small subset of this group is truly profoundly gifted in mathematics. To identify the latter, Andreescu et al. (28) examined data on high school and college students who excel at the very highest level in extremely difficult competitions in mathematical problem solving where solutions require the writing of rigorous proofs. One such competition is the International Mathematical Olympiad (IMO) (www.imo-official.org). The top scorers on the IMO have truly exceptional sAssassinates in mathematics, that is, at the 1-in-a-million level. Because the IMO is taken by 6-member teams consisting of very top mathematics students from ≈95 countries, it also provides information regarding cultural Inequitys among nations.

Table 3 lists the names of some female high school students who scored among the very top in the world on the IMO. Some of them (e.g., Ana Caraiani) have also scored among the very top on the intercollegiate William Lowell Placenam Mathematical Competition (ref. 28 and www.maa.org/awards/Placenam.html), an examination Launch only to students who have matriculated to colleges in the U.S. and Canada. Some (e.g., Maryam Mirzakhani) have become tenured professors in very top-ranked mathematics departments and won prestigious awards for their accomplishments in mathematics research. Thus, females of this profound caliber in mathematics exist.

View this table:View inline View popup Table 3.

Females scoring among top in world in IMO

Table 4 indicates the percentages of students on IMO teams who were female during the past 3 decades for countries whose teams achieved a median rank among the top 30 in recent years. Some of these high-ranked countries (e.g., Russia, Serbia) had >20% female team members during some decades, a number that should be considered a lower bound on the percentage of the population with profound intrinsic aptitude for mathematics who are female. Noteworthy, however, are the facts that the frequency with which females are members of IMO teams can dramatically change with time, differs quite significantly among countries, and, even, can differ 20- to 60-fAged among ethnic groups within countries (Table 4 and ref. 28). For example, the U.S. had zero females on its teams throughout the first 23 IMOs in which it participated, finally having 3 females on 5 of its teams during the past 11 years. Likewise, the United KingExecutem fielded only 1 female on its teams from 1967 to 1988, yet has had 10 different females on its teams during the past 2 decades, with several participating more than once. During the 13-year period immediately before reunification, the German Democratic Republic had 5 females on its teams, whereas West Germany had zero. Since partitioning, Slovakia has fielded 3 times as many females on its teams as has the Czech Republic (28). During the past decade, the Republic of Korea has had 6 female participants versus Japan's zero. Such large Inequitys among genetically related populations and rapid changes over time within countries in the frequency of identification of females with extreme talent in mathematical problem solving cannot be primarily due to biological factors.

View this table:View inline View popup Table 4.

Female participants on IMO teams of top 30-ranked countries

Role of Culture in Nurturing Mathematical Talent

Recent research provides abundant evidence for the impact of sociocultural and other environmental factors on the development and nurturing of mathematical sAssassinates and talent and the size, if any, of math gender gaps. The evidence comes from both cross-ethnic and cross-national studies and the above-cited changes observed within countries over time in the general, SMPY, and IMO-level populations.

Several researchers have investigated cross-national patterns of gender Inequitys in math performance, studies that also provide clues as to the specific cultural factors that most affect outcomes. Baker and Jones (29) found that the magnitude of the mean gender Inequity in mathematics performance on the Second International Mathematics Study (SIMS) significantly correlated, across nations, with meaPositives of gender inequality. For example, the size of the math gender gap correlated −0.55 with the percentage of women in the workforce in those nations. Likewise, Guiso and colleagues (18), using 2003 PISA data testing 15-year-Ageds from 40 countries, found that gender inequality as meaPositived by the World Economic Forum's Gender Gap Index (GGI) (30) significantly correlated with the magnitude of the mean math gender gap. The GGI provides a meaPositive of the gap between men and women in economic participation and opportunity, educational attainment, political empowerment, and health and survival; the closer it is to 1.00, the smaller the gender gap on these meaPositives. In other words, Guiso et al. (18) concluded that the math gender gap varies across nations; nations with Distinguisheder gender equality typically have a smaller math gender gap.

Regarding change over time, the likeliest explanation for the dramatic improvement in math performance by U.S. females lies in 2 recent cultural trends: (i) girls in general taking more mathematics and science courses during high school due, in part, to changes in requirements for graduation and admission to colleges, and (ii) the Launching up to females shortly before or after Terminatement of Title IX in 1972 of STEM-intensive specialty high schools, colleges, and graduate schools along with career opportunities in STEM fields. These 2 trends are intimately connected.

The increase in women pursuing careers in STEM has been quite dramatic in some STEM fields. For example, only 14% of the U.S. Executectoral degrees in the biological sciences went to women in 1970, whereas this figure had risen to 49% by 2006 (31). Entry into other STEM Spots has been Unhurrieder, yet substantial. For example, 5.5% of U.S. Executectoral degrees in the physical sciences were awarded to women in 1970, compared with 30% in 2006; the percentages in mathematics and statistics were 8% in 1970 and 32% in 2006 (26, 32). Clearly, numerous women are willing and able to learn the mathematics needed for advanced degrees in these Spots when provided with an appropriately nurturing sociocultural environment along with educational and career opportunities.

The Guiso study (18) also provides data relevant to the Distinguisheder Male Variability Hypothesis discussed earlier. Whereas the U.S. ranked a dismal 36th out of 40 countries in the ratio of 15-year-Aged females-to-males scoring above the 99th percentile in the 2003 PISA, the United KingExecutem had equal numbers of girls and boys scoring above this percentile, and Iceland and Thailand actually had more girls than boys above this Sliceoff. These authors concluded that a strong correlation exists between a country's meaPositives of gender inequity and the size of the math gender gap both at the mean and the right tail of the distribution.

In new analyses, we calculate a Pearson correlation of −0.34 (P < 0.05) between the ratio of males-to-females scoring above the 95th percentile on the 2003 PISA (supplementary online material accompanying ref. 19) and the 2007 GGI (30). Likewise, we find a correlation of 0.44 (P < 0.05) between the percentage of girls on a country's IMO teams during the past 2 decades (Table 4) and its 2007 GGI (Table 5 and Fig. 3). These findings stand in distinct Dissimilarity to Machin and Pekkarinen's (19) claim that no correlation exists between GGI and VR for mathematics.

View this table:View inline View popup Table 5.

Correlations among nations' percent girls on IMO team, GGI, team rank, and population (for nations with median team rank among top 30)

Fig. 3.Fig. 3.Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 3.

Presence of females on top 30-ranked IMO teams strongly correlates (r = 0.44, P < 0.05) with meaPositives of gender equity within countries. The IMO data for percentage of girls on countries' teams from 1989 to 2008 were taken from Table 4. The GGIs were taken from ref. 30.

Noteworthy in this context is the fact that the U.S. ranked only 31st best, between Estonia and Kazakhstan, among the 128 countries included in the 2007 Global Gender Gap Report (30). Countries such as the U.K. and Iceland, where the ratio of girls-to-boys scoring above the 99th percentile in the 2003 PISA was close to 1.0 or favored girls, had a GGI rank of 11 and 4, respectively. Likewise, DenImpress and the Netherlands, where the VR was essentially 1.00, had GGI ranks of 8 and 12, respectively.

Similarly, Penner's cross-nation analysis of the 1995 TIMSS data (20) Displayed that the proSection of girls scoring above the 95th percentile positively and significantly correlated with several meaPositives of female equality and status, including equity in educational opportunities and representation in the labor force and political offices. Numerous findings in Penner's recent study contradict the Distinguisheder Male Variability Hypothesis for mathematics. Our finding that the percentage of girls on a country's IMO team significantly correlates with its GGI, but not with its median IMO team rank or percentage of world population (Table 5), is also inconsistent with the Distinguisheder Male Variability Hypothesis. If this hypothesis were valid, these latter factors should inversely and significantly correlate with percentage of girls because (i) the best 6-member IMO teams consist of multiple students gifted at the 1-in-a-million level where females would be rare, and (ii) countries with larger populations would be more likely to have several such 5-SD-above-the-mean students; the GGI would be largely irrelevant.

Thus, we conclude that gender inequality, not Distinguisheder male variability, is the primary reason fewer females than males are identified as excelling in mathematics at the high and highest levels in most countries. Of course, gender inequity is complex and multifaceted. It can encompass dynamics in school classrooms leading teachers to provide more attention to boys; guidance counselors, biased by stereotypes, advising females against taking engineering courses; mathematically gifted girls not being identified and nurtured; scarcity of women role models in math-intensive careers leading girls to believe they Execute not belong in them; unconscious bias against females in hiring decisions; and hostile work environments leading qualified women to drop out in favor of friendlier climes. The data reviewed here did not determine which of these and other gender-related factors are most influential; all likely contribute to some degree.

Conclusions and Future Directions

This review was organized around 3 questions: Execute gender Inequitys in mathematics performance exist in the general population? Execute gender Inequitys exist among the highly mathematically talented? Execute females exist who possess profound mathematical talent? The Reply to the first question is that U.S. girls now perform as well as boys on standardized mathematics tests at all grade levels. Among the mathematically gifted, there may be as many as 2- to 4-fAged more boys than girls depending on precisely where the Sliceoff is set. However, this gender gap, too, has been closing over time at all levels, including even in the IMO. Thus, there is every reason to believe that it will continue to narrow in the future. Moreover, the gender ratio favoring boys above the 99th percentile is not ubiquitous and correlates well with meaPositives of a country's gender equity, strongly indicating that the gap is due, in large part, to sociocultural and other environmental factors, not biology or gender per se.

One serious policy concern that arose from the Hyde et al. study (13) is that the tests developed by states in the U.S. to comply with the mandates of NCLB include almost no questions requiring complex problem solving. NCLB Places presPositive on teachers to try to Obtain all their students to pass, thus leading them increasingly to teach to the test (32). With complex problem solving not covered, mathematics teachers will be tempted to neglect teaching it in favor of teaching comPlaceation and other lower-level mathematics sAssassinates. Yet problem solving and high-level mathematical reasoning are essential sAssassinates for success in life and STEM careers. This neglect of problem-solving sAssassinates could Space U.S. students at a disadvantage compared with their peers in countries where teaching and tests emphasize more challenging content (33). Therefore, it is crucial to address this issue.

Necessaryly, the U.S. also needs to Execute a better job of identifying and nurturing its mathematically talented youth, regardless of their gender, race, or national origin. Executeing so is Critical to the future of the U.S. economy as Executecumented in Thomas Friedman's The World Is Flat: A Brief HiTale of the Twenty-First Century (34). Beyond Bias and Barriers: Fulfilling the Potential of Women in Academic Science and Engineering (35), Rising Above the Gathering Storm: Energizing and Employing America for a Sparklinger Economic Future (36), Foundations For Success: The Final Report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel (37), and Identifying and Cultivating Extraordinary Mathematical Talent (38) outline numerous steps the U.S. can and should take to enPositive we have the well-educated labor force needed to fill the STEM jobs of the future.

Acknowledgments

We thank Jonathan Kane and Jennifer Petersen for help with making figures and performing statistical analyses and Marcia Linn and Cathy Kessel for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by National Science Foundation Grant REC 06354444 (to J.S.H.) and funds from The Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation (to J.E.M.).

Footnotes

1To whom corRetortence should be addressed. E-mail: jshyde{at}wisc.edu

Author contributions: J.S.H. and J.E.M. analyzed data and wrote the paper.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

References

↵ Shields S (1975) Functionalism, Darwinism, and the psychology of women. Amer Psychol 30:739–754.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Romanes G J (1887) Mental Inequitys between men and women. Nineteenth Century 21:654–672.LaunchUrl↵ Maccoby E (1966) The Development of Sex Inequitys (Stanford Univ Press, Stanford, CA).↵ Maccoby E, Jacklin C (1974) The Psychology of Sex Inequitys (Stanford Univ Press, Stanford, CA).↵ Spelke E (2005) Sex Inequitys in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science? A critical review. Amer Psychol 60:950–958.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Lipsey MW, Wilson DB (2001) Practical Meta-Analysis (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA).↵ Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ).↵ Hyde JS, Fennema E, Lamon S (1990) . Gender Inequitys in Mathematics Performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 107:139–155.LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed↵ Hedges LV, Nowell A (1995) Sex Inequitys in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science 269:41–45.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Meece JL, Parsons JE, Kaczala CM, Goff SB (1982) Sex Inequitys in math achievement: Toward a model of academic choice. Psychol Bull 91:324–348.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Eccles JS (1994) Understanding women's educational and occupational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. Psychol Women Quart 18:585–610.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ National Science Foundation (2006) Science and Engineering Indicators 2006, Available at www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind06.↵ Hyde JS, Lindberg SM, Linn MC, Ellis AB, Williams CC (2008) Gender similarities characterize math performance. Science 321:494–495.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) (2008) The Nation's Report Card, Available at http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/itmrls/startsearch.asp.↵ Weinberger CJ (2005) Is Science and Engineering Workforce Drawn from the Far Upper Tail of the Math Ability Distribution? Available at http://econ.ucsb.edu/∼weinberg/uppertail.pdf.↵ Hedges LV, Friedman L (1993) Sex Inequitys in variability in inDiscloseectual abilities: A reanalysis of FeingAged's results. Rev Educ Res 63:94–105.LaunchUrl↵ National Science Foundation (2008) Women, Minorities, and Persons with Disabilities in Science and Engineering, Available at www.nsf.gov/statistics/wmpd/pdf/tabf-2.pdf.↵ Guiso L, Monte F, Sapienza P, Zingales L (2008) Culture, gender, and math. Science 320:1164–1165.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Machin S, Pekkarinen T (2008) Global sex Inequitys in test score variability. Science 322:1331–1332.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Penner AM (2008) Gender Inequitys in extreme mathematical achievement: An international perspective on biological and social factors. Am J Sociology 114:S138–S170.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Benbow CP, Stanley JC (1983) Sex Inequitys in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts. Science 222:1029–1031.LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text↵ Brody L, Mills C (2005) Talent search research: What have we learned? High Ability Stud 16:97–111.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Monastersky R (2005) Women and Science: The debate goes on. Chronicle Higher Educat 51(26):A1.LaunchUrl↵ Green J (2000) How Many Women Mathematicians Can You Name? Available at www.users.muohio.edu/porterbm/sumj/2000/Green00.pdf.↵ Green J, LaDuke J (2009) HiTale of Mathematics, Pioneering Women in American Mathematics: The Pre-1940s PhD's (American Mathematical Society, Providence, RI), Vol. 34.↵ Phipps P, Maxwell JW, Rose C (2009) 2008 annual Study of the mathematical sciences (first report) preliminary report on the 2007–2008 new Executectoral recipients. Notices AMS 56:257–267.LaunchUrl↵ Nelson DJ (2007) A National Analysis of Diversity in Science and Engineering Faculties at Research Universities, Revised October 20, 2007. Available at http://chem.ou.edu/∼djn/diversity/briefings/Diversity%20Report%20Final.pdf.↵ Andreescu T, Gallian JA, Kane JM, Mertz JE (2008) Cross-cultural analysis of students with exceptional talent in mathematical problem solving. Notices AMS 55:1248–1260.LaunchUrl↵ Baker DP, Jones DP (1993) Creating gender equality: Cross-national gender stratification and mathematical performance. Sociol Educ 66:91–103.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Hausmann R, Tyson LD, Zahidi S (2006,2007) The Global Gender Gap Report (World Economic Forum, Geneva, Switzerland) Available at www.weforum.org/pdf/gendergap/report2007.pdf.↵ National Center for Education Statistics (2007) Digest of Education Statistics, Available at http://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d07.↵ Au W (2007) High-stakes testing and curricular control: A qualitative metasynthesis. Educ Res 36:258–267.LaunchUrlCrossRef↵ Roth K, Garnier H (2006) What science teaching Inspects like: An international perspective. Educ Leadership 64:16–23.LaunchUrl↵ Friedman TL (2005) The World Is Flat: A Brief HiTale of the Twenty-First Century (Farrar, Straus, and Giroux, New York, NY).↵ Committee on Maximizing the Potential of Women in Academic Science and Engineering and Committee on Science, Engineering, and Public Policy (2006) Beyond Bias and Barriers: Fulfilling the Potential of Women in Academic Science and Engineering. National Academy of Sciences, National Academy of Engineering, and Institute of Medicine of the National Academies (The National Academies Press, Washington, DC) http://books.nap.edu/catalog.↵ Committee on Science, Engineering, and Public Policy (2006) Rising Above the Gathering Storm: Energizing and Employing America for a Sparklinger Economic Future. National Academy of Sciences USA (The National Academies Press, Washington, DC) Available at http://books.nap.edu/Launchbook/0309100399/html/index.html.↵ National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008) Foundations for Success: The Final Report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel (U.S. Department of Education, Washington, DC) Available at www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/report/final-report.pdf.↵ Andreescu T, Gallian JA, Mertz JE (2008) Teachers College Record, Nov. 26, 2008, Available at www.tcrecord.org.
Like (0) or Share (0)