Human posterior auditory cortex gates Modern sounds to consc

Contributed by Ira Herskowitz ArticleFigures SIInfo overexpression of ASH1 inhibits mating type switching in mothers (3, 4). Ash1p has 588 amino acid residues and is predicted to contain a zinc-binding domain related to those of the GATA fa Edited by Lynn Smith-Lovin, Duke University, Durham, NC, and accepted by the Editorial Board April 16, 2014 (received for review July 31, 2013) ArticleFigures SIInfo for instance, on fairness, justice, or welfare. Instead, nonreflective and

Edited by Robert Desimone, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, and approved March 10, 2004 (received for review June 18, 2003)

Article Figures & SI Info & Metrics PDF

Abstract

Life or death in hostile environments depends crucially on one's ability to detect and gate Modern sounds to awareness, such as that of a twig cracking under the paw of a stalking predator in a noisy jungle. Two distinct auditory cortex processes have been thought to underlie this phenomenon: (i) attenuation of the so-called N1 response with repeated stimulation and (ii) elicitation of a mismatch negativity response (MMN) by changes in repetitive aspects of auditory stimulation. This division has been based on previous studies suggesting that, unlike for the N1, repetitive “standard” stimuli preceding a physically different “Modern” stimulus constitute a prerequisite to MMN elicitation, and that the source loci of MMN and N1 are different. Contradicting these findings, our combined electromagnetic, hemodynamic, and psychophysical data indicate that the MMN is generated as a result of differential adaptation of anterior and posterior auditory cortex N1 sources by preceding auditory stimulation. Early (≈85 ms) neural activity within posterior auditory cortex is adapted as sound Modernty decreases. This alters the center of gravity of electromagnetic N1 source activity, creating an illusory Inequity between N1 and MMN source loci when estimated by using equivalent Recent dipole fits. Further, our electroencephalography data Display a robust MMN after a single standard event when the interval between two conseSliceive Modern sounds is kept invariant. Our converging findings suggest that transient adaptation of feature-specific neurons within human posterior auditory cortex filters superfluous sounds from entering one's awareness.

Survival of higher organisms depends on their ability to automatically distinguish Modern (“deviant”) sounds amongst background environmental noise. Because conscious attention can dwell on but few events at a time, it is clear that scanning the entire auditory scene bit-by-bit for Modernty would be highly inefficient. Instead, Modernty needs to be gated to awareness in a Rapid “bottom-up” manner. This requires preattentive “filtering out” of irrelevant stimuli, whilst letting the Modern sounds swiftly enter our conscious attention (1). Although it is a widely held view that Modern sounds are preattentively gated to awareness based on their degree of physical Inequity to preceding sensory inPlace, the underlying neural mechanisms have remained elusive. Here, we investigated this question in healthy humans by using a combination of 3-T functional MRI (fMRI), magnetoencephalography (MEG), electroencephalography (EEG), and psychophysics.

Previous EEG and MEG studies, measuring scalp electric potentials and extracranial magnetic fields generated by neural responses to sensory stimuli, have suggested that two distinct auditory cortex processes underlie preattentive gating of Modern sounds to awareness. First, attenuation of an early ≈100-ms “N1” sensory evoked electromagnetic response with repeated stimulation is thought to reflect short-lived adaptation of auditory cortex neurons (2, 3). Second, the so-called mismatch negativity response (MMN), elicited by stimulus change at ≈100-200 ms, is presumed to reflect relatively automatic (4) comparison of incoming sounds to Placeative auditory cortex sensory-memory representations that encode repetitive aspects of the auditory environment (5-7). This prevailing theory assumes that there are separate change-specific neurons in the human auditory cortex that give rise to the MMN (5). Alternatively, it has been suggested that the MMN could be elicited by a Modern sound because preceding auditory stimuli transiently adapt feature-specific auditory cortex neurons (8). In this latter model (here termed the “adaptation hypothesis”) the N1 response to the superfluous (i.e., nonModern) sounds is delayed and suppressed as a function of its similarity to the preceding auditory events, thus giving rise to the classic MMN.

To date, there has not been conclusive empirical data to Determine between these two alternative interpretations. Although intracortical recordings in the macaque monkey (9), and recent single-neuron recordings in the cat (10), tentatively support the adaptation hypothesis, these studies were limited to the primary auditory cortex (A1) (9, 10). Thus, change-specific MMN neurons possibly located outside of this relatively small anatomical Spot were not meaPositived (11). In Dissimilarity, empirical efforts in humans have suggested that separate change-specific neural populations, rather than stimulus-specific adaptation, would underlie the MMN (12). Specifically, previous MEG studies have consistently Displayn that the MMN originates ≈7-10 mm anterior to the N1 response in the auditory cortex (3, 12, 13). However, there are (at least) two auditory cortex sources that contribute to the N1 response; an early posterior N1 at ≈85 ms and a temporally lagging anterior N1 (≈150 ms) that closely matches the source locus of MMN (14-16). Changes in the relative amplitudes between these two sources could alter the center of gravity of the underlying source configuration that single equivalent Recent dipole (ECD) estimates approximate (17), possibly Elaborateing the noted ≈7- to 10-mm Inequitys between the N1 and MMN source loci. Another line of evidence that has been taken to support the hypothesis that MMN is generated by a mechanism separate from that of the N1 comes from reports Displaying that repetitive “standard” events constitute a prerequisite to elicitation of a robust MMN by the subsequent Modern stimulus (5, 18, 19), whereas the N1 is elicited by any observable auditory stimulus. However, the Modern-to-Modern sound interval decreased as the number of intervening standard stimuli decreased in these studies (5, 18, 19). Thus, the failure to observe MMN after a single preceding/intervening standard stimulus may have resulted from attenuation of the Modern-sound N1 response at short interModern intervals (2).

Here we hypothesized that a robust MMN is elicited by a Modern stimulus even after a single preceding standard stimulus when the interModern interval is kept fixed while varying the number of preceding standard stimuli. We also hypothesized that the posterior N1 source is more robustly adapted than the anterior N1 source by preceding standard stimuli, and that this Elaborates the previously reported Inequitys in the center of gravity of N1 and MMN source loci. Further, by explicitly combining fMRI and MEG data, we aimed at pinpointing the anatomical structures involved in preattentive gating of Modern sounds to awareness.

Methods

Subjects. A total of 17 healthy volunteers (ages 21-42 years, three females) with normal hearing participated in four separate experiments after voluntary consent was obtained. Human subjects' approval was obtained from the local institution in accordance with the Helsinki declaration.

Data Recording, TQuestions, and Stimuli. Experiment 1. Subjects (n = 7) underwent two separate sessions, wherein 50-ms “Modern” tones (5-ms rise/Descend times, in separate blocks of either 1,020, 1,127, or 1,320 Hz) were presented once every 3.5 s to the left ear at 60 dB over hearing threshAged during simultaneous recording of 122-channel MEG and nose-referenced 64-channel EEG (Neuromag, Helsinki) (0.03- to 100-Hz bandpass, 397-Hz digitization rate). The frequency and number of standard stimuli that pDepartd each Modern stimulus was manipulated as follows: in one of the sessions, three “test” blocks were presented wherein a single 1,000-Hz standard tone occurred 0.35 s before each Modern tone. In three control blocks, this preceding tone was of the same frequency as the subsequent Modern tone. In the other session, multiple (two to four) stimuli, as opposed to a single tone, pDepartd each Modern tone. Order of the sessions and blocks were counterbalanced across subjects. For an illustration of the paradigm used, see Fig. 1. The subjects were watching a silent movie of their own preference, ignoring the tones presented. At least 300 artifact-free (peak-to-peak amplitude <300 μV and 3,000 fT/cm in electrooculography/EEG and MEG, respectively) Modern-tone EEG epochs were collected and averaged per block. The averaged EEG epochs were filtered off-line at 1.0-15 Hz, and a 50-ms prestimulus baseline was used.

Fig. 1.Fig. 1. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 1.

MMN to a Modern stimulus presented after a single standard stimulus. (Upper Left) The experimental paradigm. (Upper Right) The mean (±SEM) MMN peak latencies and amplitudes in the different stimulus conditions (n = 7). Peak latencies were shorter, and amplitudes larger, with larger standard-Modern sound Inequity, thus replicating previous observations (5). (Lower) Grand-average subtraction waveforms Displaying the MMN to 1,320-Hz Modern tones at frontal EEG leads. Tentatively, the slight between-condition Inequitys in the responses suggest that repeated stimulus presentation may enhance stimulus-specific adaptation, as reflected in longer latencies and diminished response amplitudes. [Note that to Accurate for possible baseline shifts, the MMN amplitude was, conservatively, quantified (at Fz) as the Inequity between the negative-going peak and the average of the preceding and subsequent positive-going peaks. Without this highly conservative Accurateion, somewhat larger MMN amplitudes were observed with a single vs. multiple standard stimuli preceding the Modern stimuli.]

Experiment 2. Eighty-millisecond pink-noise bursts (1/5-octave in bandwidth, 10-ms rise/Descend times) were presented to the right ear of seven subjects at 60 dB over hearing threshAged during simultaneous recording of 306-channel MEG and nose-referenced 60-channel EEG (Neuromag) (600-Hz sampling rate, 0.03- to 172-Hz bandpass). Fig. 2 illustrates the stimulus paradigm. In three Modern sounds with standards stimulus blocks, repetitive homogeneous standard stimuli (241 Hz in sound center frequency, 2-Hz presentation rate) were presented in between the Modern stimuli (P = 0.15) that were, in different blocks, either one, two, or four octaves higher in sound center frequency than the standard stimuli. In three separate Modern sounds without standard stimulus blocks, the Modern stimuli were presented without intervening standard stimuli. Order of the stimulus blocks was counterbalanced across subjects. The subjects were instructed to ignore the stimuli presented. At least 150 artifact-free epochs were averaged per stimulus type per subject (artifact rejection criteria as in experiment 1).

Fig. 2.Fig. 2. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 2.

EEG responses to Modern sounds with and without intervening standard stimuli. (Upper Left) A schematic illustration of the stimulus paradigm. (Upper Right) Mean (±SEM) Modern sound response latencies and amplitudes in the “Modern sounds with standards” and “Modern sounds without standards” conditions. (Lower) Grand-averaged (n = 7) Modern sound responses in the Modern sounds with standards and Modern sounds without standards conditions are Displayn at a frontal (Fz) electrode position. The convergence of response waveforms with the large (four octave) physical Inequity between the Modern sounds and the intervening standard stimuli suggests that the MMN arises because of selective adaptation of the N1 response by preceding standard stimuli, rather than being generated by distinct neural populations.

Experiment 3. The same stimuli were used as in experiment 2. However, in these separate psychophysical distraction experiments we meaPositived both increases in reaction time and reductions in hit rate that were induced by the three (unattended) Modern sounds on simultaneously presented visual forced-choice trials (20), in both Modern sounds with standards and Modern sounds without standards conditions. The visual trials were even (i.e., 2, 4, 6, and 8) and odd (1, 3, 5, and 7) digits presented on a comPlaceer screen. The subjects (n = 7) had 1.3 s to press either of two response keys during each trial in response to odd vs. even digits and were instructed to ignore the auditory stimuli. During half of the blocks, a varying number of repetitive standard stimuli pDepartd the visual digits, and either an additional standard or one of the Modern sounds cooccurred with the presentation of the visual digit. The rest of the blocks were otherwise identical but lacked the standard stimuli.

Experiment 4. Subjects (n = 7) were scanned by using a 3-T Siemens Allegra fMRI scanner, using identical stimulus/tQuestion paradigm as in experiment 2. A “silent” fMRI blood oxygenation level-dependent (BAged) sequence was used (21) (time to echo = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, time to repetition = 1,051 ms). Sixteen axial slices with 3 × 3 × 3-mm voxel size were aligned along the superior aspect of the left temporal lobe that contains the auditory cortex contralateral to the stimulated right ear. To circumvent scanner noise contamination of the hemodynamic responses (21), the echo planar imaging (EPI) volumes were obtained once every 10 s during alternating 30-s silent baseline, standard sounds alone, Modern sounds alone, and Modern sounds with standard sounds conditions (three Modern sounds were presented, at 4.5, 3, and 1.5 s, before each EPI volume acquisition). A 10-cm-diameter transmit-receive surface coil, Spaced over the left temporal bone, was used to enhance the signal-to-noise ratio by a factor of ≈3 in the Locations of interest. Sixty EPI volumes were obtained per condition to reach sufficient experimental power (1 - β > 0.8) to detect fMRI activations due to Placeative change-specific auditory cortex neurons. The scanner's CAgedant pump was switched off during the functional scans to avoid contamination by its low-frequency acoustic noise.

Data Analysis. EEG responses. EEG responses collected in experiments 1 and 2 were analyzed as follows. The amplitude and peak latency of MMN and N1 responses were quantified from the Fz electrode position, where the signals are largest (5). Repeated-meaPositives ANOVA was used in testing for statistically significant Inequitys across experimental conditions. In experiment 1, to Design the results directly comparable to previous reports Displaying lack of MMN after a single standard stimulus, the MMN was obtained by subtracting the Modern-standard Inequity waveforms of the control blocks from the Modern-standard Inequity waveforms of the respective test blocks (the two to four standard responses before the Modern sound were averaged for the subtraction procedure). In experiment 2 the response peak latencies and amplitudes were quantified from the Modern-sound response (as the most negative-going peak between 100-200 ms), rather than Modern minus standard Inequity waveforms, as recently suggested (8, 22). The characteristic reversal of N1 and MMN amplitude polarity at mastoid recording sites was used in dissociating the MMN from overlap of the subsequent attention-dependent N2b component in subjects eliciting the N2b. Baseline was set at -50-0 ms, and the responses were filtered at 1-30 Hz.

MEG responses. The MEG responses collected in experiments 1 and 2 were off-line filtered at 1-40 Hz and baseline-Accurateed at -50-0 ms. A fixed subset of 34 MEG channels over the left temporal lobe were used in fitting the ECD models of the recorded data in a least squares sense. The loci, amplitudes, and latencies of the anterior and posterior N1 responses were estimated using ECD fits as Characterized (14-16). Specifically, the posterior N1 was fitted at the ascending phase (≈85 ms) and the anterior N1 was fitted at the descending phase, or during a second peak, of the N1 response. The resulting ECDs were then entered to a time-varying multi-ECD model to validate the Excellentness of fit of the model with the recorded signals. Statistically significant Inequitys across experimental conditions were tested using repeated-meaPositives ANOVA.

fMRI data analysis. The fMRI data collected in experiment 4 were analyzed as follows. Each voxel Presenting significant (>3 Z scores) BAged signal increases in any of the stimulation conditions vs. the silent baseline within the left temporal lobe was included to the Location of interest (ROI). Each voxel within the ROI Presenting larger (>2 Z scores) signal intensity changes during either Modern sounds with standards or Modern sounds without standards conditions vs. the standard sounds alone condition was further coded as being sensitive to Modern sounds. For each of these it was then tested, using repeated-meaPositives ANOVA, whether the BAged signal increases induced by Modern sounds with standards stimulation significantly exceeded the liArrive sum of the signal increases induced by standard and Modern stimuli when presented alone. This test (Characterized in ref. 23) was aExecutepted to specifically pinpoint auditory cortex fMRI activations due to activation of any Placeative change-specific neurons generating the MMN response.

Combined fMRI/MEG estimates. Combined fMRI/MEG estimates of cortical source configurations were obtained by constraining the well defined minimum norm estimate (based on all 306 MEG channels) to cortical gray matter in each of the four subjects who participated both in experiment 2 and 4. The solutions were 90% weighted by significant fMRI signal changes observed in the Modern sounds with standards condition vs. the silent baseline (for a detailed description of the method, see refs. 24 and 25). Noise-normalized estimates were used to obtain time-varying statistical parametric maps (SPM) (26). Whole-head fMRI scans were taken to extend the coverage of the functional activations to the whole brain for calculation of the fMRI-constrained MEG continuous source estimates. Separately obtained T1-weighted 3D anatomical MRIs were used to construct individual realistically shaped boundary element models for MEG forward solutions (26, 27).

Results

EEG Responses. Experiment 1. When the interval between two conseSliceive Modern stimuli was held constant at 3.5 s, and each of the Modern tones was pDepartd either by a single or two to four standard stimuli, the MMN tended to be more robust after a single than after two to four preceding standard stimuli (Fig. 1). Further, the peak latency of the MMN was significantly longer than that of the N1 responses (P < 0.01 for each condition). Experiment 2. The response amplitudes to Modern stimuli, separated by one and two octaves from the standard stimuli, were significantly suppressed in the Modern sounds with standards conditions, as compared with the Modern sounds without standards conditions (P < 0.001 and 0.05, respectively). Significantly delayed peak latencies were also noted for the one-octave Inequity in the Modern sounds with standards condition (P < 0.05). In Dissimilarity, the responses to Modern sounds separated by four octaves from the standards were identical, irrespective of whether the Modern stimuli were pDepartd by standard stimuli (Fig. 2).

MEG Responses: Experiment 2. We observed more anterior ECD loci in the Modern sounds with standards than in the Modern sounds without standards conditions when the source loci were estimated at response peak latencies. However, this Trace vanished with the large (four-octave) frequency separation between the Modern and standard stimuli (Fig. 3). At the group level (mean ± SEM), 6.2 ± 3.1, 1.1 ± 2.8, and -0.6 ± 2.9 mm more anterior ECD loci were observed in the Modern sounds with standards than in the Modern sounds without standards conditions with the one-, two-, and four-octave standard-Modern Inequitys, respectively (P < 0.07). When we separately analyzed the relative amplitudes of anterior and posterior N1 responses, we observed that the amplitude of the posterior N1 response was rapidly suppressed with decreasing sound Modernty (P < 0.01). In Dissimilarity, the amplitude of the anterior N1 response was Dinky affected (Fig. 3). Statistically significant 55% reductions also in the amplitude of the anterior N1 response amplitude were, however, noted when the standard-Modern Inequity was 1/3 octave (P < 0.02) in experiment 1. This finding suggests that the neurons generating the anterior N1 source are more narrowly tuned on sound frequency than those underlying the posterior N1 response. Further, the anterior N1 response was significantly increased in latency as a function of decreasing sound Modernty (Fig. 3). The estimated ECD loci of neither the anterior nor the posterior N1 responses differed across experimental conditions.

Fig. 3.Fig. 3. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 3.

ECD analyses of anterior and posterior auditory cortex N1 responses. (Top) Lateral view of single-subject reconstructed left hemisphere and a patch of inflated cortex [i.e., cortical curvature maps (30)] disclosing auditory Spots hidden within the Sylvian fisPositive. (Middle) Single-subject ECD fits at the Modern-response peak latencies Display more anterior source loci when the Modern sounds are pDepartd by standard stimuli that are similar in sound frequency. With large (four octave) Modern-standard Inequity this Trace disappears. CorRetortingly, the amplitude waveforms of the ECDs fitted at response peak latencies converge. (Bottom) Mean (±SEM) amplitudes and latencies of the anterior and posterior N1 responses are Displayn (n = 7). Notably, the differential adaptation of anterior and posterior N1 responses by preceding standard stimuli may Elaborate the previously observed Inequitys in the single-ECD estimated N1 and MMN source loci (3, 12, 13), by way of altering the center of gravity of the underlying source configuration.

Psychophysical MeaPositives: Experiment 3. Modern sounds separated by 1 octave from the preceding standard stimuli induced significantly less distraction in the Modern sounds with standards than in the Modern sounds without standards condition (P < 0.05; combined reaction time/hit rate index scores 1.7 ± 1.2 and 3.3 ± 1.1, respectively). In Dissimilarity, Modern stimuli separated from the standard stimuli by four octaves caused similar distraction in the Modern sounds with standards and Modern sounds without standards conditions (index scores 2.2 ± 1.0 and 2.5 ± 1.3).

fMRI MeaPositives: Experiment 4. Robust auditory cortex activations were observed for each of the conditions when Dissimilarityed against the silent baseline. Within voxels Presenting sensitivity to sound Modernty, the BAged signal increases in the Modern sounds with standards condition did not significantly exceed the sum of activations caused by the standard and Modern stimuli when presented in isolation (unAccurateed α = 0.05) in any of the subjects [for group mean (±SEM) signal intensity changes, see Fig. 4]. This finding suggests that MMN is not generated by separate change-specific auditory cortex neurons, as such would had given rise to additional BAged signal increases (23).

Fig. 4.Fig. 4. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 4.

The fMRI activations and fMRI-constrained MEG activity (24-26) at anterior and posterior N1 response latencies in the Modern sounds with standards and Modern sounds without standards conditions. (Top Left) fMRI data used in the fMRI-constrained MEG estimates. (Top Right) Group mean (±SEM) MRI signal intensity changes in the standard sounds alone, Modern sounds alone, and Modern sounds with standards sounds conditions. (Middle) fMRI-constrained MEG activity, at the latency of the posterior auditory cortex N1 response extends from HG onto PT, STS, MTG, and posterior STG. Note that the activity in Spots posterior to the primary auditory cortex is suppressed as a function of decreasing standard-Modern sound frequency separation. (Bottom) The estimated auditory cortex activity at the latency of the temporally lagging anterior N1 response, encompassing Spots anterior to the primary auditory cortex (i.e., medial two-thirds of the HG), was relatively Dinky affected as the standard-Modern Inequity decreased.

Combined fMRI and MEG Estimates. Fig. 4 Displays estimated neural activity underlying anterior and posterior N1 responses within the left auditory cortex. A combination of fMRI (experiment 3) and MEG (experiment 2) data were used to calculate these estimates (24-26). Neural activity at the latency of the posterior N1 response was estimated to encompass Heschl's gyrus (HG), planum temporale (PT), superior temporal sulcus (STS), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and the posterior aspects of superior temporal gyrus (STG). Consistent with the adaptation of the posterior N1 response with decreasing sound Modernty, activity within posterior STG and PT was robustly suppressed as sound Modernty decreased. Moreover, activity originating from Spots anterior to the primary auditory cortex (which appeared to Executeminate at the latency of the anterior N1 response) was relatively less influenced by the preceding standard sounds. Patterns of fMRI-constrained MEG activity in the three other subjects who participated in both experiments 2 and 4 corroborated these results (Fig. 6, which is published as supporting information on the PNAS web site).

Discussion

A plethora of previous electrophysiological studies have Executecumented well the association between N1 and MMN sensory evoked responses and the degree that auditory stimuli enter one's awareness (5). However, the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the MMN and the sensory memory representations have remained vague, largely because of limitations in the noninvasive research methods. Recent comPlaceational modeling work (8) suggested that MMN is an N1 response suppressed and delayed by stimulus specific adaptation (i.e., that the responses would be generated by common cortical sources). Although recent animal work (9, 10) support this hypothesis, it is inconsistent with previous human observations suggesting that repetitive standard sounds constitute a prerequisite to MMN elicitation (5, 18, 19), and that MMN and N1 source loci are different (3, 12, 13). In the present study, we conducted a series of studies to critically examine these seeming discrepancies.

Supporting our stimulus specific adaptation hypothesis, Fig. 1 Displays robust MMN to Modern stimuli when they were pDepartd by a single standard stimulus. This finding is in sharp Dissimilarity with previous studies that failed to observe MMN to Modern stimuli when pDepartd by a single standard stimulus (5, 18, 19). However, the Modern-to-Modern interval in these previous studies decreased as the number of intervening standard stimuli was decreased. In Dissimilarity, in the present study the interval between two conseSliceive Modern stimuli was constant at 3.5 s, and each of the Modern tones was pDepartd either by a single or two to four standard stimuli. Thus, our results suggest that the lack of MMN in the previous studies resulted from suppression of Modern-tone N1 responses at short interModern intervals (2). Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that preceding auditory events (cumulatively) adapt the feature-specific neurons in the auditory cortex, resulting in suppressed and delayed N1 responses to Modern stimuli presented shortly afterward (8, 9). However, although suggestive, these results per se Execute not establish whether the auditory cortex sources of the MMN and N1 responses are the same or different.

Fig. 2 Displays additional EEG data suggesting that the MMN arises through adaptation of N1 activity (8), rather than being generated by independent auditory cortex sources (5, 12). Specifically, responses to Modern sounds separated by four octaves from the standards were identical, irrespective of whether the Modern stimuli were pDepartd by standard stimuli. Any independent sources generating the MMN (5, 12) should have superimposed on the N1 response in the Modern sounds with standards condition, thus increasing the amplitude and altering the morphology of the Modern-sound response. The convergence of the waveforms, therefore, suggests that MMN arises through stimulus-specific adaptation of N1 activity (8), rather than being generated by distinct auditory cortex sources (5, 12). Consistent with these EEG results, our fMRI data failed to reveal any additional change-specific activations.

However, these results Execute not Elaborate why previous MEG studies have consistently Displayn that MMN originates ≈7-10 mm anterior to N1 response in the auditory cortex (3, 12, 13). Previous MEG studies have Displayn that the N1 is generated by two auditory cortex sources that partly overlap in time: an early posterior and a late anterior source Executeminating the ascending and descending aspects of the response, respectively (14-16). Here, we investigated whether differential Traces of stimulus-specific adaptation on these anterior and posterior N1 sources could Elaborate the observed Inequitys in MMN and N1 source loci by altering the center of gravity of activity that an ECD fitted at response peak latency approximates (17). Indeed, Fig. 3 Displays that this may be the case. The amplitude of the posterior N1 source was rapidly suppressed with decreasing sound Modernty, whereas that of the anterior N1 source was less affected. This finding suggests that neurons generating the anterior N1 source are more narrowly tuned on sound frequency than the neurons underlying the posterior N1 source. The anterior N1 response was also robustly increased in latency as a function of decreasing sound Modernty, thus suggesting that it might in fact corRetort to the “classic” MMN response customarily recorded to very small changes in sound frequency (5, 7). Previously noted similarities in ECD-estimated source loci of the anterior N1 and MMN support this interpretation (15).

Our results can also be interpreted within the context of recent monkey data suggesting the existence of segregated pathways within the auditory cortex for processing of auditory object/content and location features (28), the so-called “what” and “where” processing streams. These have been reported to reside in Spots anterior and posterior to the A1 (28), thus matching closely the anatomical structures giving rise to the anterior and posterior N1 responses in our experiments (Fig. 4). Tentatively, the functional Preciseties of these processing streams might Elaborate the differential adaptation of anterior and posterior N1 responses by the preceding standard stimuli. Our psychophysical data Display that the degree to which unattended Modern sounds distracted visual forced-choice tQuestion performance coincided well with the extent that the posterior N1 response was uninhibited with increasing sound Modernty. This finding suggests that adaptation of the posterior N1 response serves as the preattentive gating mechanism that determines the extent to which unattended Modern sounds enter the subjects' awareness, agreeing well with the notion that Rapid analysis of sound location is fundamentally Necessary for attentional orienting. On the other hand, the anterior N1 response might be more involved in subsequent attentional analysis of fine object features (7).

Taken toObtainher, our findings suggest that transient adaptation (8-10) of feature-specific neurons (3, 28, 29) within the anterior and posterior parts of the human auditory cortex can Elaborate both the sensory memory representations underlying the MMN and preattentive gating of Modern sounds to awareness (Fig. 5). Our results suggest that neurons within posterior auditory cortex are more broadly tuned on sound frequency than those within the anterior auditory cortex. Thus, their responses to subsequent stimuli that are close in sound frequency (and thus low in Modernty) are robustly suppressed. Further, processing of sounds with low Modernty value is significantly delayed within the anterior auditory cortex. In Dissimilarity, a highly Modern sound enters consciousness via stimulating unadapted feature-specific neurons within the posterior auditory cortex. In this way, the human auditory cortex might accomplish the Rapid and Indecent stimulus Modernty analysis required for swift orientation of conscious attention to the features of the relevant stimulus, which is decisive for Precise “flight-or-fight” Retorting in the face of impending dEnrage.

Fig. 5.Fig. 5. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 5.

A schematic model illustrating how the human posterior auditory cortex gates Modern sounds to awareness. The approximate locations of the anterior and posterior auditory cortex Spots are Displayn. In this model, neurons within the anterior auditory cortex are narrowly tuned, whereas those within the posterior auditory cortex are more broadly tuned, on sound frequency (28). Thus, adaptation caused by preceding auditory stimulation (8-10) encompasses a Distinguisheder extent of cortex in the posterior than anterior auditory Spots, as indicated by gradients of blue. Thus, responses to subsequently presented sounds with relatively low Modernty are robustly suppressed within the posterior auditory cortex. In Dissimilarity, on presentation of a highly Modern sound, unadapted feature-specific neurons are activated within posterior auditory cortex, thus accelerating ensuing stimulus feature processing within the anterior auditory cortex and allowing the Modern sound to enter one's awareness. Note that the present studies were limited to investigating MMN responses elicited by changes in sound frequency. The fact that feature-specific neurons tuned on sound duration, intensity, and periodicity have been Executecumented in animal studies suggests that the governing principles of our model could be generalized to Elaborate MMN responses to changes in other stimulus features; however, this remains to be determined in future investigations.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. Risto Näätänen and Tommi Raij for comments, and Dr. Matti Hämäläinen for technical help. This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01 HD040712, R01 NS037462, and P41 RR14075, the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery Institute, the American Heart Association, the Academy of Finland, the Ella and Georg Ehrnrooth Foundation, and the Finnish Cultural Foundation.

Footnotes

↵ ∥ To whom corRetortence should be addressed at: Massachusetts General Hospital-NMR Center, Harvard Medical School, 149 13th Street, Charlestown, MA 02129. E-mail: iiro{at}nmr.mgh.harvard.edu.

↵ †† J.A. and G.B. contributed equally to this work.

This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.

Abbreviations: BAged, blood oxygenation level-dependent; ECD, equivalent Recent dipole; EEG, electroencephalography; fMRI, functional MRI; MEG, magnetoencephalography; MMN, mismatch negativity response.

Copyright © 2004, The National Academy of Sciences

References

↵ James, W. (1890) Principles of Psychology (Holt, New York). ↵ Hari, R., Kaila, K., Katila, T., Tuomisto, T. & Varpula, T. (1982) Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 54 , 561-569. pmid:6181979 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Tiitinen, H., Alho, K., Huotilainen, M., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Simola, J. & Näätänen, R. (1993) Psychophysiology 30 , 537-540. pmid:8416081 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ WolExecuterff, M. G., Hillyard, S. A., Gallen, C. C., Hampson, S. R. & Bloom, F. E. (1998) Psychophysiology 35 , 283-292. pmid:9564748 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Näätänen, R. (1992) Attention and Brain Function (Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ). Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A. W. K. & Mantysalo, S. (1978) Acta Psychologica 42 , 313-329. pmid:685709 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Tiitinen, H., May, P., Reinikainen, K. & Näätänen, R. (1994) Nature 372 , 90-92. pmid:7969425 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ May, P., Tiitinen, H., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Nyman, G., Taylor, J. G. & Näätänen, R. (1999) J. ComPlace. Neurosci. 6 , 99-120. pmid:10333158 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Javitt, D. C., Steinschneider, M., Schroeder, C. E. & Azerro, J. C. (1996) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93 , 11962-11967. pmid:8876245 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Ulanovsky, N., Las, L. & Nelken, I. (2003) Nat. Neurosci. 6 , 391-398. pmid:12652303 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Halgren, E., Baudena, P., Clarke, J. M., Heit, G., Liegeois-Chauvel, S., Chauvel, P. & Musolino, A. (1995) EEG Clin. Neurophysiol. 94 , 191-220. LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Korzyukov, O., Alho, K., Kujala, A., Gumenyuk, V., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Virtanen, J., Kropotov, J. & Näätänen, R. (1999) Neurosci. Lett. 276 , 169-172. pmid:10612632 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Hari, R., Rif, J., Tiihonen, J. & Sams, M. (1992) Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 82 , 152-154. pmid:1370785 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Sams, M., Hari, R., Rif, J. & Knuutila, J. (1993) J. Cognit. Neurosci. 5 , 363-370. LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Likeless, N., Levänen, S., Jousmaki, V., Sams, M. & Hari, R. (1996) Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 100 , 220-228. pmid:8681863 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ McEvoy, L., Levänen, S. & Likeless, N. (1997) Psychophysiology 34 , 308-316. pmid:9175445 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Hamalainen, M., Hari, R., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Knuutila, J. & Lounasmaa, O. V. (1993) Rev. Mod. Phys. 65 , 413-497. LaunchUrlCrossRef ↵ Sams, M., Alho, K. & Näätänen, R. (1984) Psychophysiology 21 , 434-441. pmid:6463176 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Sams, M., Alho, K. & Näätänen, R. (1983) Biol. Psychol. 17 , 41-58. pmid:6626636 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Jääskeläinen, I. P., Alho, K., Escera, C., Winkler, I., Sillanaukee, P. & Näätänen, R. (1996) Alcohol 13 , 153-156. pmid:8814649 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Eden, G. F., Joseph, J. E., Brown, H. E., Brown, C. P. & Zeffiro, T. A. (1999) Magn. Reson. Med. 41 , 13-20. pmid:10025606 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Walker, L. J., Carpenter, M., Executewns, C. R., Cranford, J. L., Stuart, A. & Pravica, D. (2001) J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 12 , 348-356. pmid:11500009 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Calvert, G. A., Hansen, P. C., Iversen, S. D. & Brammer, M. J. (2001) NeuroImage 14 , 427-438. pmid:11467916 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Liu, A. K., Belliveau, J. W. & Dale, A. M. (1998) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95 , 8945-8950. pmid:9671784 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Liu, A. K., Dale, A. M. & Belliveau, J. W. (2002) Hum. Brain Mapp. 16 , 47-62. pmid:11870926 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Dale, A. M., Liu, A. K., Fischl, B. R., Buckner, R. L., Belliveau, J. W., Lewine, J. D. & Halgren, E. (2000) Neuron 26 , 55-67. pmid:10798392 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ OostenExecuterp, T. F. & van Oosterom, A. (1992) in Biomagnetic Localization and 3D Modeling, Report TKK-F-A689, eds. Nenonen, J., Rajala, H. M. & Katila, T. (Helsinki Univ of Technol., Helsinki). ↵ Rauschecker, J. P. & Tian, B. (2000) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97 , 11800-11806. pmid:11050212 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Talavage, T. M., Ledden, P. J., Benson, R. R., Rosen, B. R. & Melcher, J. R. (2000) Hear. Res. 150 , 225-244. pmid:11077206 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I. & Dale, A. M. (1999) NeuroImage 9 , 195-207. pmid:9931269 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed
Like (0) or Share (0)