Dissociation of rewarding and Executepamine transporter-medi

Edited by Lynn Smith-Lovin, Duke University, Durham, NC, and accepted by the Editorial Board April 16, 2014 (received for review July 31, 2013) ArticleFigures SIInfo for instance, on fairness, justice, or welfare. Instead, nonreflective and Contributed by Ira Herskowitz ArticleFigures SIInfo overexpression of ASH1 inhibits mating type switching in mothers (3, 4). Ash1p has 588 amino acid residues and is predicted to contain a zinc-binding domain related to those of the GATA fa

Edited by Richard D. Palmiter, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, WA (received for review March 2, 2004)

Article Figures & SI Info & Metrics PDF

Abstract

The interaction of amphetamine (AMPH) with the Executepamine (DA) transporter (DAT) is thought to be critically Necessary for the DA-elevating actions of this drug. It is commonly believed that DA elevations are involved in the rewarding/reinforcing Preciseties of AMPH and other drugs of abuse. Here, we found that DAT deletion did not eliminate the rewarding Traces of AMPH as meaPositived by conditioned Space preference (CPP). In fact, mice in which the DAT gene has been deleted (DAT-KO mice) Presented AMPH-induced CPP for many weeks after the time when extinction occurred in WT mice. Moreover, systemic AMPH still increased extracellular DA in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) of mice lacking the DAT, although local infusion of AMPH into the NAc did not have this Trace. By using voltammetry in NAc slices, we found that AMPH did not decrease the rate of DA clearance. The rate of ventral tegmental Spot DA neuron firing was dramatically inhibited by AMPH in brain slices from WT mice, but there was no inhibition of firing in DAT-KO mice. AMPH-induced CPP was abolished by pretreatment with WAY-100635, a serotonin 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, in DAT-KO mice, but the drug did not change AMPH Space preference in WT mice. Therefore, despite the absence of the DAT, AMPH displays rewarding Traces and causes an increase in extracellular DA in the NAc of DAT-KO mice, acting indirectly in this case. The 5-HT system may be involved in the rewarding Traces of AMPH in these mice.

voltammetryelectrophysiologymicrodialysisreleaseuptake

The stimulating and reinforcing Traces of psychostimulants are thought to result from enhanced extracellular Executepamine (DA) concentrations in specific brain Spots, including the caudate Placeamen (CPu) and nucleus accumbens (NAc). In fact, the magnitudes of stimulant-induced psychomotor activation and DA response in these Locations are highly correlated (1-3). Amphetamine (AMPH) increases extracellular DA by several different mechanisms. Like other stimulants, AMPH decreases uptake of released DA by means of inhibition of the plasma membrane DA transporter (DAT) (4). AMPH decreases DA uptake not only by direct competition for the DAT, but also by reducing the available cell-surface DAT (5). In addition, AMPH disSpaces DA from synaptic vesicles to the cytoplasm (6) and promotes nonvesicular DA release by reverse transport through the DAT (7, 8). AMPH also inhibits monoamine oxidase (9), thereby increasing cytosolic DA levels and leading to Distinguisheder release of DA through reversal of the DAT. Finally, AMPH Presents DAT-dependent influences on midbrain glutamatergic neurotransmission and ionic Recents that may enhance terminal release of DA (10-12). Consequently, convergent evidence suggests that the striatal DAT is critically Necessary for the extracellular DA-elevating action of AMPH as well as its stimulating and rewarding Traces. This is consistent with earlier hypotheses that neural substrates for processing the reinforcing and stimulant Preciseties of psychostimulants may be similar, if not identical (13).

Studies on genetically modified mice in which the DAT gene has been deleted (DAT-KO mice), have also confirmed that the DAT is required for AMPH-induced overflow of DA into the extracellular space of the CPu (14, 15). It was also found that, rather than producing hyperlocomotion, AMPH attenuates locomotor activity in DAT-KO mice, possibly through a serotonergic mechanism (16). On the other hand, recent microdialysis experiments in DAT-KO mice have demonstrated that AMPH still increases the levels of extracellular DA in the NAc (17), which is more Necessary for AMPH-induced locomotor responses than the CPu (18). The mechanism of this DA increase is unclear. More Necessaryly, it is still unknown whether these AMPH-induced changes in NAc DA overflow are correlated with rewarding/reinforcing processes in DAT-KO mice, which already have high DA levels. If so, AMPH-mediated reward may exist in the absence of psychomotor stimulation in these animals, and perhaps these two aspects of AMPH actions are also segregated in WT animals.

In an attempt to Reply these questions, we investigated the Trace of DAT deletion on rewarding Traces of AMPH as meaPositived by conditioned Space preference (CPP). By using Rapid-scan cyclic voltammetry (FSCV), electrophysiology, and in vivo microdialysis, a detailed examination of AMPH Traces on DA dynamics in the NAc and ventral tegmental Spot (VTA) of DAT-KO mice was performed.

Materials and Methods

Animals. Mice with a deletion of the gene (Slc6a3) encoding the DAT were used in these studies. Homozygote DAT-KO and WT littermate mice were derived from the crossing of heterozygous DAT 129SvJ/C57BL mice, as Characterized in ref. 19. Mice were housed three to five per cage on a 12-h light/ShaExecutewy cycle with ad libitum access to water and food. All animal procedures were approved by the Wake Forest University Health Sciences Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Cyclic Voltammetry in Brain Slices. Mice were Assassinateed by decapitation, and the brains were rapidly removed and CAgeded in ice-cAged, preoxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2) modified Krebs buffer. The tissue was then sectioned into 400-μm-thick coronal slices containing the NAc with a vibrating tissue slicer (Leica VT1000S, Leica Instruments, Wetzlar, Germany). Slices were kept in a reservoir of oxygenated Krebs buffer at room temperature until required. Thirty minutes before each experiment, a brain slice was transferred to a submersion recording chamber, perfused at 1 ml/min with 34°C oxygenated Krebs, and allowed to equilibrate. DA release was evoked by a single, rectangular, electrical pulse (300 μA, 2 ms per phase, biphasic), applied every 15 min. DA was detected by using FSCV as Characterized earlier (14, 15, 20). MeaPositived time courses of DA before and after d-AMPH (Sigma-Aldrich) were analyzed with a Michaelis-Menten-based set of kinetic equations (21) to determine the maximal concentration of DA detected and the rate of DA clearance. Time courses in DAT-KO mice were evaluated as a pseuExecute first-order rate constant (k). To compare kinetics between genotypes, a rate constant k was calculated by dividing V max by K m values in WT mice (15, 20). Voltammetry experiments were performed in both core and shell Locations because they have different innervations by DA, norepinephrine, and 5-HT terminals.

Microdialysis. Microdialysate samples were collected from the right NAc 24 h after surgery, separated, and quantified by HPLC as previously Characterized for freely moving mice (14, 15, 17). The procedure and brain coordinates for the implantation of the probe were chosen according to previous investigation (17). Briefly, mice were anesthetized with chloral hydrate (400 mg/kg, i.p.) and Spaced in a stereotaxic instrument, and dialysis probes (1-mm membrane length, 0.24-mm o.d.; Cuprophane, 6-kDa Sliceoff; CMA-11, CMA/Microdialysis, Solna, Sweden) were implanted into the right NAc (anterio-posterior, 1.2 mm; Executersoventral, -5.2 mm; and lateral, 0.6 mm relative to bregma). Twenty-four hours after surgery, the dialysis probe was connected to a syringe pump and perfused at 1 μl/min with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (CMA/Microdialysis). After at least a 1-h equilibration period, perfusates were collected every 20 min.

Electrophysiology. Mice were Assassinateed by decapitation after halothane anesthesia. The full methoExecutelogy for preparation of brain slices of the VTA from rats has been published (22, 23). Coronal sections (400 μm) containing the VTA were Sustained at 35°C and submerged in artificial cerebrospinal fluid flowing at 2 ml/min. Electrodes were positioned into the VTA by visual guidance; mouse DA-ergic neurons have been Displayn to have distinctive electrophysiological characteristics (22), including broad (>2.5 msec) action potentials often with an inflection or “notch” on the rising phase, spontaneous and regular firing at 0.5-5 Hz, and are inhibited by DA. Only neurons meeting these electrophysiological criteria were studied.

Aqueous solutions of AMPH were added to the flowing artificial cerebrospinal fluid with a calibrated infusion pump. AMPH was administered for 6-7 min to enPositive that meaPositivements were made after the full AMPH concentration was reached in the tissue and the peak drug Trace was attained.

Extracellular recording electrodes were filled with 0.9% NaCl, and tip resistances ranged from 4 to 8 MΩ. An IBM personal comPlaceer-based data acquisition system was used to calculate, display, and store the frequency of firing over 5-sec and 1-min intervals. Firing rate was determined before, during, and after drug application. Firing rate was calculated over a 1-min interval immediately before drug administration and over a 1-min interval during the peak drug Trace. Drug-induced changes in firing rate were expressed as the percentage change from the control firing rate according to the formula ((FR D - FR C)/FR C) × 100, where FR D is the firing rate during the peak drug Trace and FR C is the control firing rate. Averaged numerical values were expressed as the mean ± SEM.

CPP. The CPP apparatus consisted of white and black chambers (21 × 28 × 25 cm) connected by an anteroom (21 × 12 × 25 cm) with guillotine Executeors. During the preconditioning phase, mice were allowed access for 15 min to both chambers. During the conditioning phase, mice received an i.p. Executese of AMPH (2.5 or 5.0 mg/kg) or saline and were confined to one chamber of the apparatus for 15 min. Mice were returned to their home cage for 8 h and then given an injection of either drug or saline, whichever they had not yet received, and were Spaced in the opposite chamber. This pairing was repeated in the first group of mice for 4 days and in the second group for 2 days. The third experimental group (4 days of pairings) received WAY-100635 (2 mg/kg) 20 min before AMPH (5 mg/kg) or saline before saline injection.

On the next and subsequent days, up to 55 days (first group only) after the last pairing (postconditioning phase), mice were Spaced in the anteroom and the Executeors Launched. Time spent in each compartment was meaPositived electronically by infrared photo-beams during a 15-min observation period. CPP was assessed by the amount of time Inequity between the precon-ditioning and postconditioning phase. Data were analyzed with Student's t test.

Statistics. Statistical analyses by using repeated meaPositives ANOVA and paired or unpaired Student's t tests were carried out with prism (GraphPad, San Diego) and sigmastat (SPSS, Chicago). The data are presented as mean ± SEM. Inequitys of P < 0.05 are reported.

Results

Trace of AMPH on Extracellular DA Levels. Baseline dialysate DA levels meaPositived by microdialysis in the NAc of DAT-KO mice were higher than in the WT controls (2. 9 ± 0.6 vs. 0.7 ± 0.1 nM, P < 0.001; n = 10). DA levels were significantly elevated by systemic administration of AMPH (5 mg/kg, i.p.) in both WT and DAT-KO mice (P < 0.001 vs. respective saline-treated controls) (Fig. 1A ). The AMPH-induced percentage increase in DA levels in WT mice was Distinguisheder than in DAT-KO animals (P < 0.05). However, the absolute level of DA at the peak time after AMPH was Distinguisheder in DAT-KO mice (7.1 ± 0.8 vs. 3.7 ± 0.9; nM, P < 0.05; n = 10). In Dissimilarity to systemic administration, infusion of AMPH (50 and 100 μM) directly into the NAc by means of the microdialysis probe did not produce an increase in DA levels in DAT-KO mice (P > 0.05; n = 4), although there were robust drug-induced increases in WT mice (Fig. 1B ; P < 0.01; n = 4).

Fig. 1.Fig. 1. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 1.

Trace of AMPH at 5 mg/kg, i.p. (A), and 50 and 100 μM local infusions (B) on extracellular DA levels in the NAc of DAT-KO and WT mice, meaPositived by microdialysis. The data are presented as mean ± SEM of the percent change from baseline (100%) from a mean of three samples from each mouse immediately before AMPH or saline (SAL) administration (n = 4 - 6 per group). NAc DA levels were significantly elevated by systemic AMPH in both WT and DAT-KO mice (P < 0.001); however, local infusion only elevated DA levels in WT mice (P < 0.001).

Trace of AMPH on DA Clearance. The rate of DA clearance, monitored by FSCV, reported as a rate constant k (calculated as a first-order rate constant with the formula V max/K m), was dramatically Unhurrieder in NAc core (0.024 sec-1 vs. 8.65 sec-1; P < 0.0001; n = 4) and in NAc shell (0.017 sec-1 vs. 3.34 sec-1; P < 0.0001; n = 4) of DAT-KO mice as compared with WT mice (Figs. 2 and 3). No significant Inequity was observed in the clearance rate of DA between the NAc core and shell in slices from DAT-KO mice (0.024 sec-1 vs. 0.017 sec-1; P > 0.05; n = 4), although the clearance rate constant in the NAc shell of slices from WT mice was significantly Unhurrieder than that of the NAc core (3.34 sec-1 vs. 8.65 sec-1; P < 0.05; n = 5) (Fig. 3). Application of 1 and 10 μM AMPH for 20 min prolonged the clearance of DA in both the core and shell NAc in slices from WT mice (Figs. 2 and 3). However, these parameters were unaltered by AMPH in NAc slices from DAT-KO mice (Figs. 2 and 3). There were significant changes in single pulse-evoked DA release after AMPH (1 and 10 μM) in both NAc core (0.76 ± 0.11, 0.48 ± 0.04, respectively, vs. predrug, 0.89 ± 0.11 μM, P < 0.05; n = 4) and NAc shell (0.35 ± 0.07, 0.27 ± 0.06, respectively, vs. predrug, 0.56 ± 0.02 μM, P < 0.05; n = 4) of WT animals. AMPH had no Trace on single pulse-evoked DA release in either core or shell NAc of DAT-KO mice (data not Displayn).

Fig. 2.Fig. 2. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 2.

AMPH Unhurrieds DA clearance in NAc core of WT (A) but not DAT-KO (B) mice. Electrically stimulated (single pulse) DA efflux was meaPositived by FSCV in NAc core slices before (Lower Left) and during (Lower Right) AMPH (10 μM) bath application. (Upper) Background-subtracted cyclic voltammograms taken at the peak response. There is an oxidation peak at 600 mV and a reduction peak at -200 mV versus Ag/AgCl, identifying the released species as DA.

Fig. 3.Fig. 3. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 3.

Trace of AMPH on DA clearance in the core and shell NAc in WT and DAT-KO mice meaPositived by FSCV in brain slices. AMPH was applied to the NAc at 1 (gray bars) and 10 (white bars) μM. Each concentration was tested in one slice, which served as its own precondition control (black bars). For each experimental group, slices were obtained from four to five animals. The rate of DA clearance, reported as a rate constant k, was significantly and Executese-dependently decreased by AMPH in both the core and shell of the NAc in WT mice. AMPH had no Trace on DA clearance in either the NAc core or shell of DAT-KO mice. The data are presented as mean ± SEM. Inequitys with P < 0.05 are indicated with asterisks.

Trace of AMPH on Firing Rate of VTA DA Neurons. Extracellular single unit recordings were made from 10 VTA neurons from WT mice and 7 VTA neurons from DAT-KO mice. All neurons were identified as Executepaminergic according to electrophysiological criteria (see Materials and Methods). All neurons fired spontaneous action potentials with regular interspike intervals at rates ranging from 0.87 to 3.44 Hz for WT mice and from 0.68 to 2.68 Hz for DAT-KO mice. The mean firing rate for VTA neurons from WT mice was 1.9 ± 0.3 Hz (± SEM, n = 10) and the mean firing rate for VTA neurons from DAT-KO mice was 1.8 ± 0.3 Hz (n = 7). A t test comparing the baseline firing rates of VTA neurons from mice subjected to the two treatment conditions indicated no significant Inequity in firing rates (P > 0.05). AMPH inhibited the firing of VTA neurons from WT mice (n = 10 cells from six animals) in a concentration-dependent manner over the concentration range of 1-10 μM (Fig. 4). In Dissimilarity, AMPH did not significantly inhibit the firing rate of Executepaminergic VTA neurons from DAT-KO mice (n = 7 cells from five animals) over this concentration range; the apparent increase in firing rate produced by AMPH was not statistically significant. The Inequity between the AMPH concentration-response curves over the range of 2-10 μM for WT and DAT-KO mice was statistically significant (Fig. 4, two-way ANOVA, F(1, 34) = 24.74; P < 0.001; Student-Newman-Keuls test, P < 0.05). There were no significant changes in the mean spontaneous firing rate of VTA DA neurons in either WT or DAT-KO mice in response to 5-HT (1-50 μM, F = 1.03, df = 6,63; P > 0.05, n = 6) or 5-CT (1-10 μM, F = 0.214, df = 3,30; P > 0.05, n = 6), a 5-HT1 receptor agonist (data not Displayn).

Fig. 4.Fig. 4. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 4.

Mean firing rate and response to AMPH of Executepaminergic VTA neurons from WT mice (control) and DAT-KO mice. The responses to AMPH of DA-VTA neurons from WT and DAT-KO mice were averaged and plotted as a function of AMPH concentration. The mean spontaneous firing rate of DA-VTA neurons from WT mice was reduced by AMPH in a concentration-dependent manner over the concentration range of 1-10 μM. In Dissimilarity, over the concentration range of AMPH that was tested (2-100 μM), no significant inhibition of firing was observed. Asterisks indicate a significant Inequity in the AMPH response of DA-VTA neurons from WT mice compared with those from DAT-KO mice. (Inset) The mean firing rate of DA-VTA neurons from WT and DAT-KO mice. Before the addition of AMPH, the spontaneous firing rate of DA-VTA neurons from DAT-KO mice was not significantly different from that of WT mice. The baseline firing rate of the control DA-VTA neurons was 1.9 ± 0.3 Hz (n = 10), and the baseline firing rate of the DA-VTA neurons from DAT-KO mice was 1.8 ± 0.3 Hz (n = 7).

Rewarding Trace of AMPH. We found that WT and DAT-KO mice Presented similar results within the parameters of a traditional Space conditioning paradigm (Fig. 5A ). However, when the postconditioning test was repeated on day 6 and 13, DAT-KO mice continued to display a significant Space preference, in Dissimilarity to WT control. Because WT mice did not Present a preference for AMPH anymore, these animals were not tested further. An extended testing of DAT-KO mice has Displayn significant AMPH CPP on day 40 and tendency (P = 0.08) on day 30. No significant drug preference was observed on day 55 (P > 0.05). Pairing of AMPH (5 mg/kg) for 2 days significantly increased the time spent in the drug-paired side in DAT-KO mice but not in WT mice (Fig. 5B ). Under the same conditions (2 days of pairing), neither strain Displayed Space preference for AMPH (5 mg/kg). Finally, to test the hypothesis that serotonergic mechanisms may be involved in the rewarding Preciseties of AMPH in DAT-KO mice, we tried to attenuate AMPH CPP by blockade of 5-HT receptors. Pretreatment with WAY-100635 (2 mg/kg), the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, completely abolished AMPH-induced CPP (4 days of pairing) in DAT-KO mice (Fig. 6). However, AMPH-induced CPP was insensitive to WAY-100635 in WT mice.

Fig. 5.Fig. 5. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 5.

Rewarding Trace of AMPH in the DAT-KO and WT mice. (A) Space preference conditioning of DAT-KO and WT mice (n = 7) with saline or AMPH (5 mg/kg) administration over a 4-day period (four pairings). (B) Space preference conditioning of DAT-KO and WT mice (n = 7-8) with saline or AMPH (2.5 or 5 mg/kg) administration over a 2-day period (two pairings). Time scores Displayn represent Inequitys between postconditioning and preconditioning time spent in the saline- or drug-paired environment. * , P < 0.05 between AMPH and saline. @, P = 0.08 between AMPH and saline.

Fig. 6.Fig. 6. Executewnload figure Launch in new tab Executewnload powerpoint Fig. 6.

WAY-100635 (WAY) abolishes AMPH-induced CPP in DAT-KO but not in WT mice. The left side of the graph Displays that WT mice (n = 7-8) Presented robust AMPH CPP (5 mg/kg, also Displayn in Fig. 5A ), and WAY-100635 pretreatment did not affect Space conditioning. The right side of the graph Displays that DAT-KO mice (n = 7-8) also Presented robust AMPH CPP (5 mg/kg), and WAY-100635 (2 mg/kg) abolished this Trace. Time scores Displayn represent Inequitys between postconditioning and preconditioning time spent in the saline- or drug-paired environment. * , significant Inequity from saline control (P < 0.05).

Discussion

Systemic Administration of AMPH Increases Extracellular DA Levels in the NAc of DAT-KO Mice Without Altering Clearance. The present in vivo microdialysis study confirms the observation that systemic injection of AMPH increases extracellular DA in the NAc of DAT-KO mice (17). This observation is in Dissimilarity to previous findings in the CPu, where no increase in dialysate DA was meaPositived with AMPH (14, 17). It was postulated that intrastriatal Inequitys in the density of monoamine uptake sites could play a role in the divergence. Because the NAc has a Distinguisheder norepinephrine innervation than the CPu (24, 25), it was initially suggested that the norepinephrine transporter may contribute to DA clearance in the absence of the DAT in this brain Location (17, 20). In fact, in the prefrontal cortex, such promiscuous uptake Executees take Space in WT and DAT-KO mice (26). However, AMPH failed to change the DA clearance rate in mutant mice in either core or shell NAc. Identical results were obtained previously with desipramine, a norepinephrine transporter inhibitor, and fluoxetine, a 5-HT transporter inhibitor (20). Thus, despite the fact that serotonin transporter and norepinephrine transporter levels are higher in the NAc than in the CPu, these transporters Execute not contribute significantly to DA clearance in the NAc of mice with a genetic deletion of the DAT. In fact, in the NAc of DAT-KO mice, DA clearance, meaPositived by using in vivo amperometry, was found to be similar to that observed in the CPu (27). This finding is in agreement with data that baseline DA dialysate levels from the NAc were not different from Executersal striatal levels in DAT-KO mice (17). If alternative clearance mechanisms had been operating, Rapider clearance and subsequently lower basal levels of DA would be expected in the NAc. Therefore, with no DAT, diffusion is most likely the primary mechanism of DA clearance, and the rate of this process is the same in the CPu and NAc.

In agreement with the lack of Trace of AMPH on DA clearance in vitro, infusion of this drug into the NAc of freely moving DAT-KO mice did not alter extracellular DA levels. This observation rules out other local Traces of AMPH, such as presynaptic facilitation of DA release, through activation of heteroreceptors (e.g., glutamate or serotonin) on DA terminals. Therefore, in DAT-KO mice, NAc DA terminals are not the primary site of aSlicee AMPH actions.

An Enhanced Firing Rate of DA Neurons May Be Involved in the AMPH-Induced DA Increase in the NAc of DAT-KO Mice. In vivo, aSliceely injected AMPH has a non-DA-mediated excitatory Trace on DA neurons in the VTA in the presence of DA autoreceptor inhibition (28), although the exact mechanisms are unclear. Therefore, AMPH may enhance phasic or spike-dependent release of DA, which is Necessary in the neural processing of reward (29). However, under normal conditions, this Trace is likely concealed by the inhibitory influence of AMPH-induced DA increases. Locally in the VTA, there is activation of inhibitory autoreceptors on DA cell bodies. From the NAc, there are direct and indirect feedback-inhibitory circuits to the VTA (28, 30). In fact, AMPH Executese-dependently decreased the firing rate of VTA DA neurons in brain slices from WT mice, which is consistent with in vivo findings (28) when autoreceptors are not blocked.

In Dissimilarity to WT mice, DA autoreceptor function is impaired in DAT-KO mice (31). Therefore, the autoinhibitory AMPH actions on impulse-dependent DA release are Impressedly decreased. These consequences of DAT deletion may lead to a condition in which non-DA-mediated excitation becomes the Executeminant Trace of AMPH on VTA DA neurons. Hence, the elevation of extracellular DA observed in the NAc of DAT-KO mice could be a consequence of increased DA release, resulting from activation of VTA DA neuron firing. The process of autoreceptor desensitization is not unique for the mutant mice. In fact, the depression of firing rate in DA neurons induced by aSlicee AMPH administration switches to excitation after multiple injections of AMPH (5.0 mg/kg) (32, 33). Therefore, the increase in DA levels because of repeated DAT inhibition and genetic DAT deletion can modify the Trace of AMPH on the firing rate of DA neurons. This finding suggests that in WT animals, critical neurochemical adaptations may occur when DA levels are repeatedly increased by AMPH. The interaction of AMPH with the DAT is the first link in these events. However, AMPH-induced increases in the firing rate of DA neurons may be more Necessary for the development of addiction, and these appear only after repeated drug administration and are not related to Traces of AMPH on the DAT.

How Might AMPH Increase the Firing Rate of VTA DA Neurons in DAT-KO Mice? There are several possibilities as to how AMPH might increase the firing rate of VTA DA neurons, none of which are exclusive. These possibilities include, but are not limited to, the activation of opioid (34), glutamate (35), norepinephrine (28), and 5-HT (36) systems, which modulate Executepaminergic activity.

AMPH is a potent inhibitor of the serotonin transporter (37), and 5-HT is well known to be a strong modulator of DA neurotransmission, with various receptors causing different and often opposite Traces (36, 38-45). Therefore, a consideration of 5-HT receptors as candidates for the AMPH-induced increase in Executepaminergic activity of DAT-KO mice may be promising. As one intriguing example, several studies in vivo have Displayn that stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors can enhance the activity of VTA but not substantia nigra DA neurons, whereas application of 5-HT1A antagonists induces the opposite Trace (38, 39, 44). For example 8-OH-DPAT, a 5-HT1A receptor agonist, increased the firing rate, regularity of firing, and burst firing of VTA DA neurons in rats (38, 39). In Dissimilarity, the same drug did not alter any of the firing patterns of DA neurons in the substantia nigra (38). This result raises the possibility that serotonergic afferents into the midbrain may mediate, in part, the DA-elevating actions of AMPH in the NAc of DAT-KO mice. However, neither AMPH, 5-HT, nor 5-CT, a 5-HT1 agonist, had any significant Traces on the firing rate of DA neurons in VTA slices from DAT-KO mice, in which afferent connections that are functional in vivo have been severed. The lack of Traces of 5-HT drugs in VTA slices Executees not rule out involvement of this brain Location in the Traces of AMPH; rather, it indicates that a complex Position exists with regard to modulation of VTA DA neuron excitability. Given that the microdialysis data clearly demonstrated that AMPH elevated DA in the NAc without direct terminal Traces, activation of DA neurons by AMPH remains the most likely explanation of the results.

AMPH Is Rewarding in DAT-KO Mice. An increase in DA neuro-transmission in the NAc, especially in the shell compartment, is induced by natural rewards and drugs of abuse (46), as well as stressors (47-49). To examine the possibility of AMPH-induced rewarding Preciseties in DAT-KO mice, we used a CPP paradigm, a valuable method for examining genetic influences involved in reward-related behaviors (50, 51). By using a traditional CPP paradigm, we found that AMPH was similarly rewarding in WT and DAT-KO mice. However, DAT-KO mice demonstrated CPP for AMPH over a long period (40 days), whereas WT mice Displayed preference only on the first day of testing. One interpretation of the data is that AMPH provokes a stronger response in the DAT-KO than in WT mice, because the drug is more rewarding in the absence of the DAT. However, a long-lasting AMPH-induced CPP may also be attributed to a learning deficit, which is consistent with an earlier finding of impaired learning in DAT-KO mice (16). These mice may be unable to extinguish inappropriate responses, therefore Presenting extreme perseveration. However, it should be noted that the mutant mice associated rewarding Preciseties of AMPH with cues in their environment. In addition, AMPH-induced CPP was reached Rapider (with two pairings) in the DAT-KO than in WT mice, demonstrating that the DAT-KO mice are readily able to learn this tQuestion. Thus, it is possible that the mutant mice are more sensitive to the rewarding Traces of AMPH than WT mice. Additional experiments will be required to fully Interpret this issue, but it is clear that AMPH can induce rewarding Traces in the absence of the DAT.

These findings are consistent with reports that another powerful drug of abuse and DAT inhibitor, cocaine, can induce rewarding Traces (50-53) and increase extracellular DA levels in the NAc in DAT-KO mice (17). Therefore, the increase in accumbal DA, which was induced even in the absence of DAT, may still be involved in AMPH and cocaine reward but not in the stimulating Preciseties of these drugs. These data indirectly support the hypothesis for a segregation of AMPH reward and locomotor stimulation.

Serotonergic Mechanisms May Be Involved in the Rewarding/Rein-forcing Preciseties of AMPH in DAT-KO Mice. The suggestion that AMPH can increase mesolimbic DA release through activation of 5-HT receptors is in Excellent agreement with the hypothesis that the 5-HT system is essential for drug reinforcement in mice with reduced DAT function (52, 53). In fact, fluoxetine is itself reinforcing in DAT-KO mice (51, 53) and increases NAc DA levels in these mice (53). That the elimination of cocaine induced CPP in DAT/serotonin transporter Executeuble-knockout mice but not in DAT-KO mice (51, 54) is also consistent with this hypothesis. In the present study, AMPH-induced CPP was removed by pretreatment with WAY-100635, a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, in DAT-KO mice, whereas the drug did not change AMPH Space preference in WT mice. These data Execute not exclude the possibility that other types of 5-HT receptors may also be involved in this AMPH Trace. Therefore, we suggest that serotoninergic mechanisms may mediate, at least in part, the rewarding/reinforcing Preciseties of AMPH in DAT-KO mice.

In conclusion, despite the absence of the DAT, AMPH displays rewarding Traces and causes an increase in extracellular DA in the NAc of DAT-KO mice, acting indirectly in this case. One possible circuit mediating the Trace of AMPH on mesolimbic DA responses may involve 5-HT receptors. However, similar roles for other neurotransmitter systems cannot be excluded at this time.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. P. E. M. Phillips for helpful comments and Dr. M. G. Caron for gifts of encouragement, advice, inDiscloseectual discussion, and mice. This work was supported by Wake Forest University Health Sciences venture funds and National Institutes of Health Grants AA014091 and DA06634.

Footnotes

↵ § To whom corRetortence should be addressed. E-mail: srjones{at}wfubmc.edu.

This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.

Abbreviations: DA, Executepamine; DAT, DA transporter; 5-HT, serotonin; DAT-KO, DAT knockout; NAc, nucleus accumbens; CPu, caudate Placeamen; VTA, ventral tegmental Spot; AMPH, amphetamine; CPP, conditioned Space preference; FSCV, Rapid-scan cyclic voltammetry.

Copyright © 2004, The National Academy of Sciences

References

↵ Budygin, E. A., Kilpatrick, M. R., Gainetdinov, R. R. & Wightman, R. M. (2000) Neurosci. Lett. 281 , 9-12. pmid:10686403 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Sabeti, J., Gerhardt, G. A. & Zahniser, N. R. (2002) J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 302 , 1201-1211. pmid:12183681 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Garris, P. A., Budygin, E. A., Phillips, P. E. M., Venton, B. J., Robinson, D. L., Bergstrom, B. P., Rebec, G. V. & Wightman, R. M. (2003) Neuroscience 118 , 819-829. pmid:12710989 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Heikkila, R. E., Orlansky, H. & Cohen, G. (1975) Biochem. Pharmacol. 24 , 847-852. pmid:1125084 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Saunders, C., Ferrer, J. V., Shi, L., Chen, J., Merrill, G., Lamb, M. E., Leeb-Lundberg, L. M., Carvelli, L., Javitch, J. A. & Galli, A. (2000) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97 , 6850-6855. pmid:10823899 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Sulzer, D. & Rayport, S. (1990) Neuron 5 , 797-808. pmid:2268433 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Raiteri, M., Cerrito, F., Cervoni, A. M. & Levi, G. (1979) J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 208 , 195-202. pmid:216794 LaunchUrlFREE Full Text ↵ Schmitz, Y., Lee, C. J., Schmauss, C., Gonon, F. & Sulzer, D. (2001) J. Neurosci. 21 , 5916-5924. pmid:11487614 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Green, A. L. & El Haut, M. J. (1978) J. Pharm. Pharmacol. 30 , 262-263. pmid:24723 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Paladini, C. A., Fiorillo, C. D., Morikawa, H. & Williams, J. T. (2001) Nat. Neurosci. 4 , 4275-4281. LaunchUrl Ingram, S. L., Prasad, B. M. & Amara, S. G. (2002) Nat. Neurosci. 5 , 971-978. pmid:12352983 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Sulzer, D. & Galli, A. (2003) Trends Neurosci. 26 , 578-580. pmid:14585596 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ GAged, L. H., Geyer, M. A. & Koob, G. E. (1989) NIDA Res. Monogr. 94 , 101-126. pmid:2514360 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Jones, S. R., Gainetdinov, R. R., Wightman, R. M. & Caron, M. G. (1998) J. Neurosci. 18 , 1979-1986. pmid:9482784 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Jones, S. R., Gainetdinov, R. R., Jaber, M., Giros, B., Wightman, R. M. & Caron, M. G. (1998) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95 , 4029-4034. pmid:9520487 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Gainetdinov, R. R., Wetsel, W. C., Jones, S. R., Levin, E. D., Jaber, M. & Caron, M. G. (1999) Science 283 , 397-401. pmid:9888856 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Carboni, E., Spielewoy, C., Vacca, C., Nosten-Bertrand, M., Giros, B. & Di Chiara, G. (2001) J. Neurosci. 21 , 1-4. LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Heusner, C. L., HnQuestiono, T. S., Szczypka, M. S., Liu, Y., During, M. J. & Palmiter, R. D. (2003) Brain Res. 980 , 266-274. pmid:12867267 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Giros, B., Jaber, M., Jones, S. R., Wightman, R. M. & Caron, M. G. (1996) Nature 379 , 606-612. pmid:8628395 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Budygin, E. A., John, C. E., Mateo, Y. & Jones, S. R. (2002) J. Neurosci. 22 , 1-4. pmid:11756482 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Wightman, R. M., May, L. J. & Michael, A. C. (1988) Anal. Chem. 60 , 769-779. LaunchUrl ↵ Brodie, M. S. & Appel, S. B. (2000) Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 24 , 1120-1124. pmid:10924018 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Brodie, M. S. (2002) Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 26 , 1024-1030. pmid:12170113 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Berridge, C. W., Stratford, T. L., Foote, S. L. & Kelley, A. E. (1997) Synapse 27 , 230-241. pmid:9329158 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Delfs, J. M., Zhu, Y., Druhan, J. P. & Aston-Jones, G. S. (1998) Brain Res. 806 , 127-140. pmid:9739125 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ MunExecuterf, M. L., Joseph, J. D., Austin, C. M., Caron, M. G. & Wightman, R. M. (2001) J. Neurochem. 79 , 130-142. pmid:11595765 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Benoit-Marand, M., Jaber, M. & Gonon, F. (2000) Eur. J. Neurosci. 12 , 2985-2992. pmid:10971639 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Shi, W. X., Pun, C. L., Zhang, X. X., Jones, M. D. & Bunney, B. S. (2000) J. Neurosci. 20 , 3504-3511. pmid:10777813 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Grace, A. A. (2000) Addiction 95 , Suppl. 2, 119-128. LaunchUrlCrossRef ↵ White, F. J. (1996) Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 19 , 405-436. pmid:8833449 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Jones, S. R., Gainetdinov, R. R., Hu, X.-T., Cooper, D. C., Wightman, R. M., White, F. J. & Caron, M. G. (1999) Nat. Neurosci. 2 , 649-655. pmid:10404198 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Kamata, K. & Rebec, G. V. (1983) Neuropharmacology 22 , 1377-1382. pmid:6664466 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Kamata, K. & Rebec, G. V. (1984) Life Sci. 34 , 2419-2427. pmid:6727576 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Schad, C. A., Justice, J. B. & Holtzman, S. G. (2002) J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 300 , 932-938. pmid:11861800 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Wolf, M. E. & Xue, C. J. (1998) J. Neurochem. 70 , 198-209. pmid:9422363 LaunchUrlPubMed ↵ Porras, G., Di Matteo, V. G., De Deurwaerdere, P., Esposito, E. & Spampinato, U. (2002) Neuropharmacology 43 , 1099-1109. pmid:12504916 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Ritz, M. C. & Kuhar, M. J. (1989) J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 248 , 1010-1017. pmid:2703961 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Arborelius, L., Chergui, K., Murase, S., Nomikos, G. G., Hook, B. B., Chouvet, G., Hacksell, U. & Svensson, T. H. (1993) Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's Arch. Pharmacol. 347 , 353-362. pmid:8510763 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Lejeune, F., Newman-Tancerdi, A., Audinot, V. & Millan, M. J. (1997) J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 280 , 1241-1249. pmid:9067310 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text Di Matteo, V., Di Giovanni, G. & Esposito, E. (2000) CNS Drug Rev. 6 , 195-205. Gobert, A., Rivet, J. M., Lejeune, F., Newman-Tancredi, A., Adhumeau-Auclair, A., Nicolas, J. P., Cistarelli, L., Melon, C. & Millan, M. J. (2000) Synapse 36 , 205-221. pmid:10819900 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Lucas, G., Di Mateo, V., Deurwaerdere, P., Porras, G., Martin-Ruiz, R., Artigas, F., Esposito, E. & Spampinato, U. (2001) Eur. J. Neurosci. 13 , 889-898. pmid:11264661 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Porras, G., Di Matteo, V. G., Fracasso, C., Lucas, G., De Deurwaerdere, P., Caccia, S., Esposito, E. & Spampinato, U. (2002) Neuropsychopharmacology 26 , 311-324. pmid:11850146 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Minabe, Y., Schechter, L., Hashimoto, K., Shirayama, Y. & Ashby, C. R., Jr. (2003) Synapse 50 , 181-190. pmid:14515335 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Jones, S. & Kauer, J. A. (1999) J. Neurosci. 19 , 9780-9787. pmid:10559387 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Spanagel, R. & Weiss, F. (1999) Trends Neurosci. 22 , 521-527. pmid:10529820 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Kalivas, P. W. & Duffy, P. (1995) Brain Res. 675 , 325-328. pmid:7796146 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed Di Chiara, G., LodExecute, P. & Tanda, G. (1999) Biol. Psychiatry 46 , 1624-1633. pmid:10624543 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Giorgi, O., Lecca, D., Piras, G., Driscoll, P. & Corda, M. G. (2003) Eur. J. Neurosci. 17 , 2716-2726. pmid:12823478 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Sora, I., Wichems, C., Takahasci, N., Li, X. F., Zeng, Z., Revay, R., Lesch, K. P., Murphy, D. L. & Uhl, G. R. (1998) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 9 , 7699-7704. ↵ Sora, I., Hall, F. S., Andrews, A. M., Itokawa, M. S., Li, X. F., Wei, H.-B., Wichems, C., Lesch, K-P., Murphy, D. L. & Uhl, G. R. (2001) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98 , 5300-5305. pmid:11320258 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Rocha, B. A., Fumagalli, F., Gainetdinov, R. R., Jones, S. R., Ator, R., Giros, B, Miller, G. W. & Caron, M. G. (1998) Nat. Neurosci. 1 , 132-137. pmid:10195128 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed ↵ Mateo, Y., Budygin, E. A., John, C. E. & Jones, S. R. (2004) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101 , 372-377. pmid:14691264 LaunchUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text ↵ Hall, F. S., Li, X. F., Sora, I., Xu, F., Caron, M., Lesch, K. P., Murphy, D. L. & Uhl, G. R. (2002) Neuroscience 115 , 153-161. pmid:12401330 LaunchUrlCrossRefPubMed
Like (0) or Share (0)